key: cord-0022591-xh8rt5wk authors: Fahrenthold, Berkeley K.; Cavanaugh, Matthew R.; Jang, Subin; Murphy, Allison J.; Ajina, Sara; Bridge, Holly; Huxlin, Krystel R. title: Optic Tract Shrinkage Limits Visual Restoration After Occipital Stroke date: 2021-07-16 journal: Stroke DOI: 10.1161/strokeaha.121.034738 sha: 4eacc9ecfbc8635c8312d2875d4e1daa76c06d87 doc_id: 22591 cord_uid: xh8rt5wk BACKGROUND AND PURPOSE: Damage to the adult primary visual cortex (V1) causes vision loss in the contralateral visual hemifield, initiating a process of trans-synaptic retrograde degeneration. The present study examined functional implications of this process, asking if degeneration impacted the amount of visual recovery attainable from visual restoration training in chronic patients, and if restoration training impacted optic tract (OT) shrinkage. METHODS: Magnetic resonance imaging was used to measure OT volumes bilaterally in 36 patients with unilateral occipital stroke. From OT volumes, we computed laterality indices (LI), estimating the stroke-induced OT shrinkage in each case. A subset of these chronic patients (n=14, 13±6 months poststroke) underwent an average of nearly 1 year of daily visual restoration training, which repeatedly stimulated vision in their blind field. The amount of visual field recovery was quantified using Humphrey perimetry, and post training magnetic resonance imaging was used to assess the impact of training on OT shrinkage. RESULTS: OT LI was correlated with time since stroke: it was close to 0 (no measurable OT shrinkage) in subacute participants (<6 months poststroke) while chronic participants (>6 months poststroke) exhibited LI >0, but with significant variability. Visual training did not systematically alter LI, but chronic patients with baseline LI≈0 (no OT shrinkage) exhibited greater visual field recovery than those with LI>0. CONCLUSIONS: Unilateral OT shrinkage becomes detectable with magnetic resonance imaging by ≈7 months poststroke, albeit with significant interindividual variability. Although visual restoration training did not alter the amount of degeneration already sustained, OT shrinkage appeared to serve as a biomarker of the potential for training-induced visual recovery in chronic cortically blind patients. identify, or discriminate particular features of targets presented in the blind-field can partially restore vision in CB, as measured by both clinical perimetry and psychophysical tests of visual performance. [9] [10] [11] [12] [13] [14] [15] [16] [17] [18] [19] [20] Yet, these restoration studies show considerable individual variability in the efficacy of training among CB patients, and the training required tends to be intensive, laborious, and lengthy. [9] [10] [11] [12] [13] [14] [15] [16] [17] [18] [19] [20] As we consider introducing restoration therapies clinically, it is important to understand the root causes of this variability so as to identify which patients are most likely to benefit and when. One factor likely to affect the potential to recover vision after V1 damage is the state of the residual visual system. Without intervention, the perimetrically defined visual deficit of patients >6 months poststroke slowly expands and worsens over time 10, 18, 21 -a phenomenon attributed to retrograde degeneration of early visual pathways. Studies in humans and nonhuman primates have long shown the existence of trans-synaptic retrograde degeneration in the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus and retina after occipital lesions. [22] [23] [24] [25] [26] [27] [28] [29] This is thought to occur in sequence, with dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus relay cells dying first, followed by loss of retinal ganglion cells. In macaque monkeys, the magnitude of trans-synaptic retrograde degeneration varies, appearing to correlate with the size of, as well as time since the V1 lesion. 25, 30 In humans, structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) analyses have shown that the optic tract (OT) ipsilateral to occipital cortex damage is often reduced in size, 23, 27, 28, [31] [32] [33] as are the thicknesses of the retinal ganglion cells and nerve fiber layers over corresponding regions of the retina in each eye. 28, 34 Importantly, these human studies also showed considerable interindividual variability in the severity of degeneration. We now posit that since early visual neurons are responsible for conveying the bulk of sensory input to the rest of the visual system, their loss could critically limit the ability of patients with V1 damage to recover visual functions. This was recently shown to be true with respect to spontaneous recovery, which occurs with marked individual variability during the subacute period, <6 months poststroke. 34 However, to what extent retrograde degeneration limits vision restoration induced by training interventions in the chronic (>6 months) poststroke period remains to be determined and is the primary question addressed with the present study. De-identified data are available on FigShare: https://figshare. com/s/e86de19092d149765d42 The present analyses were performed on magnetic resonance imaging data from 36 CB patients ( For each patient, we documented the nature of the stroke (ischemic or hemorrhagic), and the presence of several riskfactors, including diabetes, hypertension and migraine. Finally, we excluded patients with ocular disease (other than refractive error), neglect and neurological disease unrelated to the occipital stroke. Humphrey visual field (HVF) automated perimetry was collected in all Rochester patients save one, in one Oxford patient, and was not collected in controls. The training portion of the study was performed in a subset of chronic patients from Rochester only. Changes in HVF performance were used as the outcome measure (see Data Supplement for details of restoration training and HVF analysis protocols), which was correlated with MRI-derived OT metrics (detailed in the Data Supplement). Eligibility for the training arm of the study required that chronic CB participants meet additional criteria: (1) reliable 24-2 and 10-2 Humphrey visual field perimetry tests, with reliability defined as <20% fixation losses, false-positive and false-negative errors in either eye, (2) fixation precision better than ±1 degree relative to a fixation spot during psychophysical testing, measured using an Eyelink-1000 eye tracker (SR Research, Canada), and (3) ability to perform months of daily, in-home training, followed by a return laboratory visit in which performance on the home-training tasks was verified with fixation enforced using the Eyelink-1000 eye tracker. Only 14 of the original chronic participants met these criteria. During their return visit to the Rochester laboratory, 3 of the 14 trained patients declined (claustrophobia) or failed to meet eligibility criteria for a repeat MRI (incompatible device implantation during the training period). Thus, while we could measure training-induced changes in all 14 patients using psychophysics and HVF tests, we were unable to obtain a posttraining measure of OT laterality in these 3 patients. For University of Rochester participants, MRI data were acquired on a 3T Magnetom Trio scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) using a 32-channel head coil. High-resolution T1-weighted anatomic volumes (MP-RAGE, magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo) were collected with a voxel size of 1 mm 3 . For University of Oxford participants, MRI data were acquired with a 3T Prisma scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) at the Oxford Centre for Functional MRI of the Brain (FMRIB). Whole-head, structural, T1-weighted scans were collected axially at a resolution of 1 mm 3 (MP-RAGE; TR: 1900 ms; echo time: 3.97 ms; flip angle: 8°). The OT volume analysis was adapted from prior published work. 23, 32 To account for differences in head orientation in the scanner, FMRIB software library (FSL) image analysis software (http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl) was used to reorient the OTs parallel to the anterior-posterior axis (y axis) in standard space (1 mm), and the scans were resampled parallel and perpendicular to the OTs. Details on methodology to quantify OT shrinkage are provided in the Data Supplement, and illustrated in Figure I in the Data Supplement. Standard parametric tests (ie, repeated-measures ANOVAs, paired t-tests) were used to assess within-subject differences. For independent sample comparisons, unpaired t-tests were used when contrasting 2 groups; ANOVAs were used to contrast 3 or more groups. Partial eta-squared and Cohen d values were calculated to assess effect size for ANOVAs and t-tests, respectively. Linear regressions were used to model the relationship between explanatory variables and dependent outcomes, with r values and 95% confidence intervals (CI 95 ) for rho provided, and significance estimated using a t test. We first contrasted OT volumes in CB patients and visually intact controls, computing a laterality index (LI) for each person ( Figure 1A ). Control and subacute CB participants had an LI ≈0 for all OT pixel intensity thresholds ( Figure 1A , gray, and open red symbols, respectively). In contrast, chronic CB participants LI values ( Figure 1A , filled red symbols) became increasingly positive at higher pixel intensities. More positive LI values denoted a higher number of brighter voxels in the contralesional relative to the ipsilesional OTs. As a group, the LI max (LI value for maximal OT voxel brightness) of chronic CBs was greater than in subacute CBs (t 34 =−5.27, P<0.0001, CI 95 =±0.073, ±0.149) or in visually intact controls (t 32 =−4.6, P<0.0001, CI 95 =±0.059, ±0.149). Importantly, this was not explained by chronic patients having larger visual defect sizes (area of 24-2 HVF with a pattern deviation <−5 dB, t 30 =0.47, P=0.641, CI 95 =±89, ±127), or severity, measured by perimetric mean deviation (PMD) averaged between the 2 eyes (t 30 =1.15, P=0.259, CI 95 =±1.9, ±2.6). When considering all CB patients (subacute and chronic), time since stroke and OT laterality were positively correlated ( Figure 1B : r=0.3566, CI 95 for rho=0.032 to 0.613, t 42 = 2.23, P=0.032). LI max values in subacute CB patients were close to zero (open symbols, mean±SEM=−0.016±0.08, Figure 1B ), resembling control participants (LI max =0.043±0.02). In contrast, chronic patients had some of the highest, most positive LI max values (eg, right-most solid red symbols in Figure 1B ), albeit with a large range of LI max values evidenced as early as 7 months poststroke. To examine factors contributing to the heterogeneity of OT LI max among chronic patients, we separated them into 2 groups: those with LI max >0.256 were termed lateralized ( Figure 1C ); those with LI max <0.256 nonlateralized ( Figure 1D ). LI max of 0.256 was chosen because it is 2 SDs greater than the LI max of controls. Using this classification, mean LI max in lateralized chron-ics=0.66±0.06 (SEM), while mean LI max in nonlateralized chronics=0.01±0.04. In chronic patients, LI max was no longer correlated with time since stroke ( Figure 2A Finally, since a large volume of V1 is devoted to the representation of the central 10° vision, [35] [36] [37] [38] [39] we asked if those participants whose deficit impacted this central region were more likely to be lateralized. As shown in Figure III Figure IV in the Data Supplement). Overall, our data suggest that independent of initial deficit size, and given extensive visual training in the blind field, initial structural integrity of the OTs at training onset may impact training efficacy in chronic CB patients. Given baseline variability in LI max , we posit that baseline LI max is a key factor driving individual variability in training outcomes for this patient population. Our final question was whether visual training in the chronic period altered retrograde degeneration at the level of the OTs. Our laboratory has conducted a range of training studies where measurable improvements in visual perception were elicited in chronic CB patients. 10, 14, 40 Here, we analyzed the subset of 11 chronic CB patients who underwent ≈1 year of visual training, together with pre-and posttraining MRI. All 11 exhibited improvements that could be verified using fixation-enforced psychophysical testing and HVF perimetry; 7 of them were lateralized at baseline (ie, before training onset), and 4 were nonlateralized. Visual discrimination training in the blind field induced improvement ≥6 dB across an area of the HVF 101±24 deg 2 (SEM), which translated to a 1.3±0.3 dB increase in the binocular PMD. Consistent with earlier observations that pretraining LI max was negatively correlated with HVF improvement, nonlateralized chronic patients exhibited a larger area of HVF improvement than lateralized patients ( Table 2) , although both groups trained for a similar number of sessions (t 9 =−1.2, P=0.26, CI 95 =±174, ±545). PMD change, which reflects vision averaged across the entire visual field, was not sufficiently sensitive to detect this blind-field specific improvement with our present, small sample size ( Table 2) . To assess the effect of visual training on OT laterality, we analyzed LI 85 rather than LI max , accounting for the fact that not every patient had voxel intensities >85% of maximum at both time-points. We found no significant difference between pre-and post-training LI 85 across the 11 patients imaged ( Figure 4A The present study builds upon previous findings about retrograde degeneration following damage to the adult, occipital cortex, and the fundamental observation that this degeneration follows a distinct time course and pattern. 22, 23, 25, 28, 32 We not only show strong evidence of unilateral shrinkage of the OT ipsilateral to the damaged V1 in a large cohort of chronic CB patients but also highlight the lack of measurable OT shrinkage in subacute patients. Differences in deficit size were ruled out as a possible explanation, confirming the notion 28,41 that trans-synaptic, retrograde degeneration must require ≈6 months post-V1 stroke to be evidenced at the level of the human OTs. A clear observation in the present study was that chronic CB patients are not a uniform population in terms of OT degeneration, with 60% showing significant OT lateralization, but 39% having LI max similar to visually intact controls (and subacute CB patients). Whether preservation of the OT -sometimes years after a stroke-in a proportion of chronic CB patients truly signifies decreased or absent retrograde degeneration remains to be determined. However, since we found no mention of this dichotomy in prior literature, 22, 23, 25, 28, 32, 33, 42, 43 we began to interrogate its possible causes. We ruled out time poststroke as a significant predictor of chronic LI max . Only one of the chronic patients in our entire sample had documented diabetes (Type 2) and they were nonlateralized. Additionally, 10/17 (59%) of the lateralized chronic patients and 7/11 (64%) of the nonlateralized chronic patients had hypertension. The incidence of migraine was similarly small across groups: 4/17 (24%) in lateralized patients and 3/11 (27%) in nonlateralized participants. All in all, in our sample, diabetes, hypertension, and migraine did not seem to be significant predictors, nor did they correlate with shrinkage of the ipsilesional OT after occipital stroke. Instead, chronic patients with larger, more severe visual deficits (measured using HVF and Goldmann perimetry) had larger LI max . Thus, greater damage to V1 caused increased degeneration of the ipsilesional OT, with deficit (and likely lesion) size accounting for much of the variance in chronic LI max . However, several other factors, which could have contributed to individual differences in retrograde degeneration after V1 damage, could not be evaluated here. We can only speculate that the relative amount of damage sustained to gray and white matter, the exact nature of treatments received in the clinic (eg, tPA), as well as genetic and epi-genetic influences on cellular repair and inflammatory responses could all influence the rate of degeneration and plastic potential of early visual pathways after occipital strokes. Nonetheless, a key question emerging was: to what extent did the degeneration sustained impact the amount of recovery these individuals could achieve with restorative interventions. A recent study 34 reported a correlation between preserved, early visual circuitry (retinal ganglion cell layer thickness and stimulus-based early visual cortex activity) and the amount and location of spontaneous visual improvements during the subacute, poststroke period. However, over the last few decades, a major focus in the field has been to investigate visual training approaches to restore vision in chronic CB patients. These patients are of interest because they no longer exhibit spontaneous recovery, 4 but intensive visual training can recover a range of visual abilities in the blind-field of some (but not all) these individuals (reviewed in study by Melnick et al, 15 Matteo et al, 44 Perez et al, 45 and Pouget et al 46 ) . Such variability in the extent of recovery was clearly evidenced in the present cohort: given comparable amounts of daily training over a long period of time, chronic CB patients with nonlateralized OTs (LI max close to 0) at the onset of training attained significantly larger HVF improvements than lateralized, chronic CB patients. Thus, baseline pretraining LI max emerges as a biomarker able to individually predict the efficacy of visual training for recovering vision in the chronic period poststroke. Several factors have been postulated as key for training-induced visual field recovery in chronic CB, including the preservation and re-activation by training of perilesional V1 cortex weakened, but not destroyed, by the stroke. 47 The present findings support this hypothesis, showing that patients with the greatest preservation of retino-geniculostriate circuits sustain the greatest perceptual benefit from visual training. This is not to say that chronic patients with greater degeneration of early circuits cannot benefit from restoration training-they can, but the amount of benefit attained for the amount of effort expended is less. Perhaps these patients' visual recovery relies to a larger extent on extra-geniculo-striate pathways that convey information to extrastriate visual cortical areas for processing. [48] [49] [50] [51] [52] [53] [54] [55] [56] Our findings would suggest that recruitment of these alternative pathways by training may be less efficient for recovering conscious vision than recruitment of residual V1. Finally, we asked if visual training administered in the chronic poststroke period changes LI. In patients with lateralized OTs, an interesting outcome would be to see training decrease LI, suggesting that training can reverse structural and/or functional processes associated with retrograde degeneration. For patients with nonlateralized OTs, an equally interesting outcome would be to see training prevent them from becoming lateralized, presumably by blocking further degeneration. All chronic patients who were trained attained some measure of visual recovery inside their blind field-even those who only began training a year or more after their occipital stroke (see also study by Huxlin and Cavanaugh, 10 Cavanaugh et al, 11 Das et al, 13 Huxlin et al, 14 Saionz et al 18 ). However, we saw no overall change in LI 85 as a function of training. Due to our reduced sample size, this result was clearest in lateralized patients (n=7), where visual training neither decreased nor increased OT laterality. Among the 4 nonlateralized patients, 1 converted to lateralized (suggesting that training did not prevent degeneration from progressing), but the remaining 3 did not. This is an extraordinarily difficult experiment to carry out: patients need to meet MRI eligibility twice-≈1year apart on average-in addition to performing rigorous, correctly executed visual training in their blind-field daily for ≈1 year on average. Nonetheless, additional studies using greater sample sizes are needed to sufficiently power any claims about the effect of training on the OTs in chronic patients. The present findings illustrate some important structural limitations of restorative plasticity in adult, chronic stroke patients. Follow-up studies that include larger and more homogenous groups of chronic patients are needed to verify the functional implications of these results. Additionally, from a rehabilitation perspective, OT shrinkage could be measured and used to identify chronic patients who may benefit from different training strategies. However, our results also highlight potential opportunities offered by intervening in the subacute rather than chronic poststroke period. Could training administered early as opposed to late after stroke, stop or slow the progress of retrograde degeneration? This makes intuitive sense, as retrograde degeneration in humans is well underway and clearly evidenced by structural changes relatively early in the chronic period. 25, 28, 34, 57 Intervening in the subacute period may catch the visual system in a state where degeneration is not yet complete. Therefore, we posit that future studies should investigate if early intervention in acute and sub-acute CB populations can slow the rate or magnitude of degeneration, keeping LI max closer to zero for longer and thus, further improving the potential for training induced visual recovery once participants reach the chronic poststroke period. Even if the progression of degeneration cannot be stemmed, an intervention during the subacute period would take advantage of the greater integrity of retinal ganglion cells and the white matter tracts (including the OT) conveying information to subcortical centers to attain larger training-induced visual improvements. We recently verified this hypothesis, showing that visual training in subacute CB elicits more rapid and spatially extensive recovery than identical training in the chronic period. 18 However, the possibility also remains that interventions started before (rather than after) retrograde degeneration is fully evidenced, may act to slow or stop its progress. If true, this would reinforce the notion that time is indeed vision after occipital strokes. More importantly, early intervention could create a larger pool of chronic patients with increased potential for further, traininginduced recovery. Homonymous hemianopia in stroke Recovery of visual fields in acute stroke: homonymous hemianopia associated with adverse prognosis Perimetric demonstration of spontaneous visual field recovery following occipital lobe haemorrhage Natural history of homonymous hemianopia Field expansion for homonymous hemianopia by optically induced peripheral exotropia Use of NeuroEyeCoach TM to improve eye movement efficacy in patients with homonymous visual field loss Visual scanning training effect on reading-related tasks in acquired right brain damage Interventions for visual field defects in people with stroke Segregation of spontaneous and training induced recovery from visual field defects in subacute stroke patients Visual discrimination training improves Humphrey perimetry in chronic cortically induced blindness Visual recovery in cortical blindness is limited by high internal noise From blindsight to sight: cognitive rehabilitation of visual field defects. Restor Neurol Neurosci Beyond blindsight: properties of visual relearning in cortically blind fields Perceptual relearning of complex visual motion after V1 damage in humans Relearning to see in cortical blindness Temporal sensitivity in a hemianopic visual field can be improved by long-term training using flicker stimulation Improved detection following neuro-eye therapy in patients with post-geniculate brain damage Functional preservation and enhanced capacity for visual restoration in subacute occipital stroke Evidence for perceptual learning with repeated stimulation after partial and total cortical blindness Reorganization of retinotopic maps after occipital lobe infarction Natural progression of perimetric visual field defects after V1 stroke Direct demonstration of transsynaptic degeneration in the human visual system: a comparison of retrograde and anterograde changes Imaging reveals optic tract degeneration in hemianopia Transneuronal retrograde degeneration of retinal ganglion cells after damage to striate cortex in macaque monkeys: selective loss of P beta cells Transneuronal retrograde degeneration of retinal ganglion cells and optic tract in hemianopic monkeys and humans Retrograde degeneration of retinal ganglion cells in homonymous hemianopsia Retrograde trans-synaptic retinal ganglion cell loss identified by optical coherence tomography The time course of retrograde trans-synaptic degeneration following occipital lobe damage in humans Transneuronal retrograde degeneration of the retinal ganglion cells in patients with cerebral infarction Variance in transneuronal retrograde ganglion cell degeneration in monkeys after removal of striate cortex: effects of size of the cortical lesion Homonymous ganglion cell layer thinning after isolated occipital lesion: macular OCT demonstrates transsynaptic retrograde retinal degeneration Quantifying the pattern of optic tract degeneration in human hemianopia Retinal ganglion cell atrophy in homonymous hemianopia due to acquired occipital lesions observed using cirrus high-definition-OCT Survival of retinal ganglion cells after damage to the occipital lobe in humans is activity dependent The representation of the visual field in human striate cortex. A revision of the classic Holmes map Preferential representation of the fovea in the primary visual cortex Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging Retinotopic organization in human visual cortex and the spatial precision of functional MRI Cortical magnification within human primary visual cortex correlates with acuity thresholds Feature-based attention potentiates recovery of fine direction discrimination in cortically blind patients Retinal ganglion cell dysfunction in humans following post-geniculate lesions: specific spatio-temporal losses revealed by pattern ERG Corresponding ganglion cell atrophy in patients with postgeniculate homonymous visual field loss Reconciling visual field defects and retinal nerve fibre layer asymmetric patterns in retrograde degeneration: an extended case series: retinal nerve fibre layer defects in retrograde degeneration Visual field restorative rehabilitation after brain injury Rehabilitation of homonymous hemianopia: insight into blindsight Acquired visual field defects rehabilitation: critical review and perspectives Spared perilesional V1 activity underlies training-induced recovery in cortically-blind patients Blindsight and unconscious vision: what they teach us about the human visual system Subcortical pathways to extrastriate visual cortex underlie residual vision following bilateral damage to V1 Rehabilitation of damage to the visual brain Human blindsight is mediated by an intact geniculo-extrastriate pathway Changes in connectivity after visual cortical brain damage underlie altered visual function Primary visual cortex: awareness and blindsight Visually driven activation in macaque areas V2 and V3 without input from the primary visual cortex Blindsight depends on the lateral geniculate nucleus Visual capacity in the hemianopic field following a restricted occipital ablation Early diffusion evidence of retrograde transsynaptic degeneration in the human visual system The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies Drs Fahrenthold, Bridge, and Huxlin designed the study; Drs Fahrenthold, Ajina, and Bridge collected data; Dr Fahrenthold, Dr Cavanaugh, S. Jang, A.J. Murphy, and Dr Huxlin analyzed the data; Drs Fahrenthold and Huxlin wrote the article, and all authors critically reviewed and commented on it. We thank Terrance Schaefer and Hannah Willis, who performed visual field tests, and Christine Callan for patient recruitment and her outstanding work as research coordinator, regulatory compliance monitor, and quality controller. Expanded Materials and Methods Online Table I Online Figures I-IV