key: cord-0015408-ebnqtyt4 authors: Neta, Maital; Brock, Rebecca L. title: Social connectedness and negative affect uniquely explain individual differences in response to emotional ambiguity date: 2021-02-16 journal: Sci Rep DOI: 10.1038/s41598-020-80471-2 sha: 0ab7a3f47bed2a7e7fcab54b26e9cb462b25a193 doc_id: 15408 cord_uid: ebnqtyt4 Negativity bias is not only central to mood and anxiety disorders, but can powerfully impact our decision-making across domains (e.g., financial, medical, social). This project builds on previous work examining negativity bias using dual-valence ambiguity. Specifically, although some facial expressions have a relatively clear negative (angry) or positive valence (happy), surprised expressions are interpreted negatively by some and positively by others, providing insight into one’s valence bias. Here, we examine putative sources of variability that distinguish individuals with a more negative versus positive valence bias using structural equation modeling. Our model reveals that one’s propensity toward negativity (operationalized as temperamental negative affect and internalizing symptomology) predicts valence bias particularly in older adulthood when a more positive bias is generally expected. Further, variability in social connectedness (a propensity to seek out social connections, use those connections to regulate one’s own emotions, and be empathic) emerges as a notable and unique predictor of valence bias, likely because these traits help to override an initial, default negativity. We argue that this task represents an important approach to examining variability in affective bias, and can be specifically useful across the lifespan and in populations with internalizing disorders or even subclinical symptomology. Valence bias. As in previous work, there was a wide range of inter-participant variability in valence bias for both the faces and scenes (see Fig. 1 ; higher scores are associated with a more negative bias). Interestingly, the ratings of ambiguous faces were more negative and also more variable across participants than ratings of scenes. As expected, valence bias across stimuli was significantly positively correlated, r = 0.22 (see Table 1 ); however, the correlation was small in magnitude. Correlations among concurrent measures were all in the expected direction ( Table 1 ). The strongest association among indicators of negative affect was between neuroticism (NEON) and trait anxiety (STAIT; r = 0.79), and the weakest was between depression symptoms (BDI) and difficulties in emotion regulation (DERS; r = 0.53). The strongest association among indicators of social connectedness was between empathy (EQ) and extraversion (NEOE; r = 0.49), and the weakest was between empathy (EQ) and interpersonal emotion regulation (IRQ; r = 0.36). As expected, average valence bias (i.e., higher values represent a more negative bias) was significantly positively correlated with depression symptoms (r = 0.07), neuroticism (r = 0. 16) , and state (r = 0.09) and trait (r = 0.11) anxiety, and negatively correlated with interpersonal emotion regulation (r = − 0.11), extraversion (r = − 0.15) and age (r = − 0.18). Table 1 . Correlations among dependent variables. ***p < 0.001 (two-tailed), **p < 0.01 (two-tailed), *p < 0.05 (two-tailed). BDI Beck Depression Inventory, DERS Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale, NEON neuroticism, STAIS state anxiety, STAIT-Trait Anxiety, EQ Empathy Quotient, IRQ Interpersonal Regulation Questionnaire, NEOE Extraversion, VB Valence Bias-higher scores associated with a more negative bias. The three factors demonstrated adequate discriminant validity; negative affect was inversely correlated with social connectedness (r = − 0.52, p < 0.001) and positively with a more negative valence bias (r = 0.19, p < 0.001), and social connectedness was inversely correlated with a more negative valence bias (r = − 0.25, p < 0.001). We also examined correlations between the three latent factors and age which were significant (see Table 2 ). Interestingly, although there was a strong inverse correlation between negative affect and social connectedness, both were also inversely correlated with age. As predicted, age was also inversely correlated with a more negative valence bias (i.e., older adults had a more positive valence bias). After establishing adequate fit of the measurement model, we tested the full hypothesized model with negative affect, social connectedness, and age (z scored for interpretation) as predictors of valence bias, but omitted the interaction between negative affect and age. This step is necessary in the context of latent moderated structural equation models which do not produce traditional model fit indices. Maslowsky et al. 57 recommend evaluating the global fit of the model without the interaction first, and then adding the interaction to examine its significance. The global fit of this model was adequate (CFI = 0.931, TLI = 0.903, RMSEA = 0.066, 90% confidence interval = 0.058-0.073, SRMR = 0.054). Next, we added the latent interaction between negative affect and age to the model which was significant, b = 0.17 (0.06), p = 0.010. Note that because there were 113 participants who had missing scores on both variables used to define the latent interaction (i.e., negative affect and age), those cases were not included in this model. Figure 2 shows the final model results (see Supplementary Table S3 for the unstandardized model solution) . As expected, social connectedness was inversely associated with a more negative valence bias when controlling for negative affect and age, β = − 0.26, p = 0.002. There was also a significant interaction between negative affect and age predicting valence bias, β = 0.16, p = 0.01, such that there was a stronger positive association between negative affect and a more negative valence bias for older individuals. To probe the significant interaction, we conducted a regions of significance analysis such that conditional effects of negative affect on valence bias were estimated at all observed levels of age, and the significance of those conditional effects were examined. Negative affect was positively associated with a more negative valence bias, controlling for social connectedness, for individuals age 51.6 and older [i.e., beginning at 1.95 SDs above the mean of age, M (SD) = 28.06 (12.07); Fig. 3 ]. Finally, based on some work showing that older age is associated with a greater investment in more rewarding and meaningful social relationships 58 , we explored a supplementary model that included the latent interaction between social connectedness and age, but this interaction was not significant, b = − 0.14 (0.09), p = 0.094. There are vast individual differences in the tendency to interpret dual-valence ambiguity as having a more positive or negative meaning. This valence bias is not only central to mood and anxiety disorders, but can powerfully impact our decision-making across domains (e.g., financial, medical, social), and thus have dramatic and widespread consequences for many aspects of our lives. To examine the underlying sources of variability that might distinguish individuals with a more negative versus positive valence bias, we conducted an analysis across fourteen experiments. Broadly, we found that both negative affect and social connectedness appear to uniquely explain individual differences in valence bias. This finding is consistent with extensive research that has demonstrated an important link between negativity bias and mood and anxiety disorders 1-3 . We have built on this link by demonstrating a relationship between valence bias and negative affect, which was conceptualized as a latent measure representing temperamental negative affect and internalizing symptomology. Although negative affect was significantly correlated with a more negative valence bias (Table 2) , upon examining its unique association with bias (i.e., controlling for social connectedness), it was only associated with valence bias for older adults. In other words, older adults (i.e., around 51 years of age and up) that were higher in negative affect showed a more negative valence bias independent from social connectedness (i.e., no such association was evident in younger adults). Interestingly, prior work on valence bias in younger adults has demonstrated that the initial or default interpretation of ambiguity is negative 23, [43] [44] [45] , and that positive interpretations require an additional regulatory process that overrides this initial negativity 47 . Other related work has shown that children show a more negative bias than adults putatively because regulatory mechanisms responsible for producing a positive bias are weaker in children Table 2 . Correlations among latent variables and age. ***p < 0.001 (two-tailed), **p < 0.01 (two-tailed), *p < 0.05 (two-tailed). www.nature.com/scientificreports/ Figure 2 . Results of the final model. Social connectedness was inversely associated with a more negative valence bias when controlling for negative affect and age. There was also evidence for a significant interaction between negative affect and age predicting valence bias, such that there was a stronger positive association between negative affect and a more negative bias for older individuals. Values along each arrow are standardized estimates, with associated standard error in parentheses, and significance level represented by asterisks. . Depiction of the significant interaction between negative affect and age. The straight line represents conditional effects of negative affect on valence bias at different ages and the curved lines reflect the 95% confidence interval for those effects. Negative affect was positively associated with a more negative valence bias, controlling for social connectedness, for individuals age 51.6 and older (i.e., beginning at 1.95 SDs above the mean of age). www.nature.com/scientificreports/ than adults 59 , see also 60 . As such, it could be that, while a negativity bias is associated with dysfunction (depression, anxiety), it is perhaps the relative failure to develop mechanisms for regulating or overriding the negativity that may serve both to maintain the negativity bias into adulthood and to increase the risk for disorders (see 61 ) . Although there is an overall association between negative affect and valence bias in the current work, we found that negative affect was not uniquely predictive of valence bias after controlling for social connectedness in young adults. This finding might be due to their default negativity. In other words, when the default response is negative, the impact of negative affect (temperament and symptomology) may be diminished. In contrast, in older adults, a positive valence bias is more likely 43, 62 and may even represent the new default 63 . Thus, in this population, increases in negative affect appear to play a more crucial role in impacting valence bias. Alternatively, it could be that the interaction with age is due to some developmental process whereby, as individuals increasingly engage with more stimuli, there are more opportunities for their underlying proclivity toward negativity to manifest in a more negative valence bias, thus resulting in a more robust and stable bias. Future longitudinal work will be needed in order to disentangle these effects across different stages of the lifespan. Indeed, it could be that there are important mediators of the relationship between valence bias and negative affect (or negativity bias per se) in young adulthood (see 64 ) . Further, implementing this developmental framework would allow us to track within-person developmental processes and examine if and how this process strengthens over time. Having said that, valence bias is measured along the full valence spectrum, from negative to positive; thus, we would be remiss to discuss the individual differences that support a negativity bias without mentioning the ramifications for positivity bias. We found that variability in social connectedness, which was conceptualized as a latent measure representing empathy, extraversion, and interpersonal emotion regulation (i.e., the tendency to rely on others in order to regulate one's own emotions), was inversely associated with a more negative valence bias. In other words, individuals higher in social connectedness showed a more positive bias. Notably, although social connectedness is predominantly conceptualized as a facet of positive emotionality, low levels of social connectedness are significantly associated with greater negative affect (see Table 2 ). Results of the SEM analyses suggest that only the truly positive features of social connectedness (unique from negative affect) are predictive of valence bias. These findings are consistent with extant work showing that extraversion is associated with greater positive affect 36 and decreased negativity (i.e., decreased mood and anxiety symptomology 40, 41 ) . Notably, our results are consistent with work showing that social connectedness increases positive emotions and well-being [48] [49] [50] and mitigates internalizing symptoms [52] [53] [54] [55] . As briefly mentioned above, one might predict that variability in positive affect may be more important in predicting valence bias than the variability in negative affect, given that the negative bias represents the default response (in young adults). In other words, although there are important individual differences that reinforce a more negative bias, the ability to override the default negativity appears to more critically rely on variability in social connectedness that helps to downregulate or overcome the tendency to view dual-valence ambiguity in a negative light. In contrast, a low propensity toward experiencing negative emotions (low sensitivity to distressing stimuli) may be insufficient to demonstrate a positive bias, at least in young adulthood. Future longitudinal work could examine the development of these social connectedness measures over the course of one's lifetime. This work could also prove useful in improving our ability to identify individuals who are at risk for maintaining this negativity bias and developing depression or anxiety (e.g., people low in social connectedness and therefore lower in positivity bias). Given the theoretical connection between valence bias and negativity bias and its associated symptomology, we explored the bivariate associations between the average valence bias and individual measures within each latent construct (negative affect, social connectedness) and age. Consistent with our predictions, a more negative valence bias was positively correlated with four of the five individual measures within the negative affect construct: depression symptoms, neuroticism, and state and trait anxiety. Further, it was correlated with two of the three individual measures within the social connectedness construct: interpersonal emotion regulation and extraversion. It is intriguing that valence bias did not show a relationship with empathy, as in previous work 23 , future research will be needed to further explore this link. As predicted, a more negative valence bias was inversely correlated with age. This finding is consistent with prior work demonstrating that increasing age is associated with a more positive valence bias 43, 62 . Interestingly, although negative affect was inversely related to social connectedness, both of these constructs were inversely related to age (i.e., older adults showed decreases in negative affect and social connectedness). Future work targeting this aging population will be helpful to explicate the mechanisms underlying these changes, specifically as they relate to a shift toward a more positive valence bias. In sum, although extant behavioral, psychophysiological, and neuroimaging research has provided important information about the valence bias, it has fallen short of elucidating the mechanisms underlying this variability. Extensive research has focused on dispositional negativity (see 65 for a review), but here, we provide a model suggesting that the variance related to positivity (that may specifically help to overcome a negativity bias) is more sensitive to the valence bias than an approach that focuses on negativity. In other words, social connectedness emerged as a unique predictor of valence bias, and variability in negativity was an important predictor only in later life, when a positive bias is generally expected. In other words, a heightened sensitivity to subtle indicators of positivity and interpersonal connections might facilitate the override of default tendencies toward negativity, but a low propensity toward negative emotions (e.g., low depression/anxiety symptomology) is insufficient for a positive bias. These results are consistent with previous work showing that specific traits are associated with the propensity to experience negative and positive affect, respectively 36 , 39 ), and differences in neural responses to emotion 34, 35 . However, the results build on existing research by establishing a methodological advance in studying valence bias that implements dual-valence ambiguity (both positive and negative information are present in the stimuli). www.nature.com/scientificreports/ These findings provide further evidence for the idea that this bias represents a relatively stable trait-like indicator of underlying individual differences 22, 23 . As such, we argue that this task represents an important approach for future work examining variability in affective bias, and can be useful in research across age and in populations with related affective disorders or even subclinical symptomology. Indeed, there are a variety of benefits of this performance-based measure of valence bias. First, although much of the literature on negativity bias has focused on comparing patients and controls (e.g., 66 ), our task has demonstrated a dimensional association between bias and subclinical depression symptomology 61 . Second, our task shows high test-retest reliability across one year, indicating that the bias is a trait-like difference across individuals 23 . It also generalizes across ratings of different stimuli, including ambiguously valenced (surprised) facial expressions, scenes, and emotionally laden words 22 , 67 . Third, it engages an amygdala-prefrontal cortex (PFC) circuitry 21, 47, 61 , similar to that implicated in disorders characterized by a negativity bias and emotion dysregulation 66, 68 . Fourth, this task is developmentally appropriate-it has been leveraged for studying valence bias and its association with depressive symptomology and emotion regulation across the lifespan (ages 6-88 years), from children-including those experiencing early life stress [59] [60] [61] to older adults 43, 62, 63 . Finally, it is sensitive to a range of contextual manipulations, including but not limited to stress 69 and exercise 70 . As a result, this task offers a novel contribution to research on negativity bias using an approach that controls the information perceived by the participants (i.e., viewing the same dual-valence images) and enables a stable measure of bias. Future work could also examine the functional outcomes of these effects. For example, given that differences in valence bias have important consequences for social and emotional function (e.g., depression/anxiety symptomology), further research should be directed at examining the utility of this model in distinguishing individuals with normal versus aberrant function. Results of this research can inform interventions by identifying individuals with heightened resilience or increased risk for affective disorders. Indeed, a positive valence bias is associated with resilience in the face of stress 71 . Further, ongoing work shows promise for emotion regulation training and mindfulness-based stress reduction see 24 , both strategies used as interventions for mood and anxiety disorders, in promoting a more positive valence bias. Given that social connectedness is a unique predictor of valence bias, and appears to support overriding the initial negativity, a specific focus on building social connections (e.g., training in interpersonal emotion regulation or loving kindness meditation; see 49 ) might be explored as a putative intervention for ameliorating a chronic negativity bias. Table S1 ). A total of 326 participants were removed from the MTurk sample for the following reasons: 183 were removed because they failed to complete at least 75% of the experiment, an additional 104 were removed because they completed the experiment in less than 300 s (which was determined to not be feasible for the number of trials included in the study), four were removed because they did not complete the valence bias task, which was crucial for the analyses in the current report, and finally, 35 were removed for having inaccurate ratings in the valence bias task (we use a standard threshold of 60% accuracy as in previous work 23, 43 ). In addition, 96 participants that completed experiments in the lab were excluded: 64 for having inaccurate ratings in the valence bias task, and an additional 32 due to technical issues. The final sample resulted in 1390 adult participants (754 female, 562 male, 74 not reported). Age data was lost for 113 participants, but for the remaining sample, the age range was 17-88 years [mean (SD) age = 28.06 (12.07)]. Race data was lost for 73 participants, but for the remaining sample, there were 2 identifying as American Indian/Alaska Native, 117 as Asian, 61 as Black/ African-American, 895 as White, 4 as Multiracial, 46 as Other, and 192 as Unknown/Choosing not to identify. None of the participants were aware of the purpose of the experiment. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and they were compensated for their participation through monetary payment or course credit. Before each session, written or electronic informed consent was obtained from all participants, with a waiver of informed consent from parents/legal guardians for minors that were enrolled as students at the University of Nebraska-Lincoln. All of the procedures were carried out in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations, and approved by the ethics committee of University of Nebraska-Lincoln for the Protection of Human Subjects. The only criteria used for selecting participants to include in the report was that they completed the valence bias task, and the same set of surveys, for use as the dependent variables in our analyses. Procedures. Across all experiments, participants first completed a valence bias task. In this task, images of faces from the NimStim 72 , Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces 73 and Umea University Database of Facial Expressions 74 sets and scenes from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS 75 ), were presented. The faces included angry, happy, and surprised expressions, and the IAPS scenes were those previously identified as having a negative, positive, or ambiguous valence,ambiguity was defined in pilot work as those images that have a high standard deviation in valence ratings (i.e., individuals were in the most disagreement about their valence 22 ). For both stimuli, previous work found that, unlike ratings of clearly negative and positive stimuli, there is a wide range of inter-participant variability in ratings of ambiguous faces and scenes 22, 23 . Participants www.nature.com/scientificreports/ set of images (faces or IAPS, which were presented in separate blocks) presented in a pseudorandom order, and block order was counterbalanced between participants. Task data were collected in several software packages: Eprime (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA, USA), MouseTracker 76 , and Qualtrics (Qualtrics, Provo, UT). Measures. Valence bias. The dependent measure used to quantify valence bias was percent negative ratings, which is calculated as the percent of trials on which a participant viewed an emotionally ambiguous stimulus and rated it as negative, out of the total number of trials for that condition (excluding omissions). Notably, previous work has demonstrated that the valence bias generalizes across faces and scenes; the same people that tend to rate surprised faces as positive also tend to rate ambiguous scenes as positive 22 . Given that these two valence bias scores should represent some stable, trait-like measure of the tendency to interpret ambiguity as having a more positive versus negative meaning, valence bias was represented by a latent construct using these two scores. This approach provides a more stable representation of valence bias that is common across the face and scene contexts. Having said that, to explore the bivariate correlations between valence bias and the other individual difference measures, we also created a bias score using the average of the face and scene ratings. This average score represents both the common and unique variance among the two measures. While most of the participants completed valence bias tasks with both stimuli, a subset of participants completed the task with only faces,for those latter individuals, the average valence bias score was treated as missing. Negative affect. We administered a series of questionnaires to assess individual differences in negative affect. In order to account for missing data for any one question on a given questionnaire, which was minimal (< 1%), replacement scores were calculated with mean imputation by scale, and after reverse coding items as needed. Depression symptomology. Scores were extracted from the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI 77 ), one of the most widely used measures to assess severity of depression symptomology. This measure has demonstrated reliability and validity in a number of studies 77 . Each of the 21 items consists of four self-evaluative statements, ranging in severity, for which respondents select the statement that best describes their symptoms from the last 2 weeks. Ratings from each item are summed, with a possible range of 0-63. In the present study, internal consistency was excellent (Cronbach's alpha = 0.898). Difficulty in emotion regulation. Scores were extracted from the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS 78 ) to assess typical levels of difficulties in emotion regulation. This measure is based on a clinically-useful conceptualization of emotion regulation that was designed to be applicable to a wide variety of psychological difficulties and relevant to treatment development for clinical populations 79 . The DERS is a 36-item, factor-analytically derived questionnaire that comprises six subscales representing unique dimensions of emotion regulation difficulties: (1) nonacceptance of emotional responses, (2) difficulty engaging in goal-directed behavior, (3) impulse control difficulties, (4) lack of emotional awareness, (5) limited access to emotion regulation strategies, and (6) lack of emotional clarity. The DERS has demonstrated excellent convergent and discriminant validity. Respondents rate how often each item applies to them using a 5-point Likert scale (1 = Almost Never, 5 = Almost Always). Ratings from each item are summed to create a composite score across the six factors, with a possible range of 36-180. In the present study, internal consistency was excellent (Cronbach's alpha = 0.948). Neuroticism. Scores were extracted from the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI 80 ) to assess neuroticism (NEON). The NEO-FFI is one of the most widely used instruments to assess personality. This 60-item questionnaire includes scales to measure the big five personality traits, and respondents rate each item on the degree to which it is true or not true of them, using a 4-point Likert scale (0 = Strongly Agree, 3 = Strongly Disagree). Due to differences in the survey procedure across studies, some participants only completed a subset of the 60-item questionnaire (e.g., neuroticism and extraversion scales only). Although the NEO-FFI has had multiple revisions since its original development in 1990, adequate reliability has been consistency demonstrated over the years 81 . The NEON scale consists of 12 items that were summed, with a possible range of 0-48. In the present study, internal consistency was excellent (Cronbach's alpha = 0.862). State and trait anxiety symptoms. Scores were extracted from the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI 82 ) to assess state and trait anxiety symptomology. State Anxiety (STAIS) is measured with a 20-item questionnaire in which respondents rate each item on the degree to which it is true or not true of them, using a 4-point Likert scale (1 = Not At All, 4 = Very Much So). Ratings from each item are summed, with a possible range of 20-80. Trait Anxiety (STAIT) is measured similarly, except respondents rate each item on the degree to which it is true or not true of how they "generally feel". In the present study, internal consistency was excellent (Cronbach's alpha: STAIS = 0.921, STAIT = 0.935). Social connectedness. We administered a series of questionnaires to assess individual differences relevant to social connectedness. Again, mean imputation was used to account for any missing data on a given questionnaire (< 1%). Empathy. Scores were extracted from the abridged Empathy Quotient (EQ 83 ) to assess variability in empathy. The EQ is a relatively recent measure of empathy that is unique in that it was explicitly designed to have clinical application 84 . The abridged EQ includes 22 items that respondents rate on the degree to which it is true or not Interpersonal emotion regulation. Scores were extracted from the Interpersonal Regulation Questionnaire (IRQ 85 ) to assess variability in one's recruitment of social resources to up-and down-regulate one's own emotions. This questionnaire includes measures of both tendency to recruit these social resources and efficacy with which these resources are perceived to be helpful, each with respect to managing both positive and negative emotions. The IRQ is a 16-item, factor-analytically derived questionnaire that is comprised of four subscales representing these unique dimensions: (1) negative emotions-tendency, (2) negative emotions-efficacy, (3) positive emotions-tendency, and (4) positive emotions-efficacy. The IRQ has demonstrated excellent convergent and discriminant validity, and is distinct from measures of negative expressivity, intrapersonal emotion regulation ability, and extraversion. Respondents rate each item on the degree to which it is true or not true of them, using a 7-point Likert scale (1 = Strongly Disagree, 7 = Strongly Agree). Ratings from each item are summed to create a composite score across the four factors, with a possible range of 16-112. In the present study, internal consistency was excellent (Cronbach's alpha = 0.932). Extraversion. Scores were extracted from the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI 80 ) to assess extraversion (NEOE). As previously described, the NEO-FFI is one of the most widely used instruments to assess personality. The NEOE scale consists of 12 items that were summed, with a possible range of 0-48. In the present study, internal consistency was excellent (Cronbach's alpha = 0.855). Age and potential controls. A series of demographic characteristics (e.g., education, sex, household income, race, ethnicity, past history of mental or physical illness, medication use) as well as methodological differences across studies (i.e., presentation duration, number of trials, response method-mouse versus keyboard, study location-online versus laboratory) were examined. However, only age consistently correlated with both measures of valence bias and at least one of the predictors (see Tables 1, 2 ) and, as such, none of the other variables were included as controls. And based on research suggesting that there may be important age-related changes in the nature of a default negative valence bias across the adult lifespan, ultimately, age was included as an interactive effect with negative affect rather than a control (see below). Statistical analyses. Data were analyzed using Mplus 86 . Because of missing data for some surveys across different experiments, some scores were missing. Covariance coverage for the model ranged from 0.137 to 0.979, but a large majority (> 80%) exceeded 0.40. Table 3 reports sample size for each variable and Supplementary Table S2 reports covariance coverage. Further, full information maximum likelihood estimation (FIML 87 ) is considered an optimal approach to addressing missing data patterns of this magnitude. Thus, by implementing FIML, we capitalized on all available data from this large dataset. Additionally, robust standard errors were estimated using the MLR estimator in Mplus to address any violations of univariate or multivariate normality. We applied a latent moderated structural equations method 57 for testing moderation hypotheses. Multiple indices were used to assess global model fit. The comparative fit index (CFI > 0.90), Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI > 0.90), root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA < 0.08), and standardized root-mean residual (SRMR < 0.08) are reported. Once a model was determined to adequately fit the data, parameter estimates were interpreted. Three latent variables were modeled. The metric of the latent variables was set by fixing the variance of the latent variables to 1.00,thus, latent scores were standardized. First, scores from the BDI, DERS, NEON, STAIS, and STAIT were modeled as indicators of a latent variable representing negative affect. Second, scores from the www.nature.com/scientificreports/ EQ, IRQ, and NEOE were modeled as indicators of a latent variable representing social connectedness. Third, valence bias for faces (VB-faces and scenes) (VB-scenes) were modeled as indicators of a latent variable representing valence bias. In this model, the latent scores of negative affect and social connectedness were used to predict valence bias. In addition, we explored interactive effects of age and negative affect, predicting that age might play a more critical role in the relationship between negative affect and valence bias given prior work demonstrating that increasing age is associated with a more positive valence bias 43, 62 . In other words, we expected that the association between negative affect and valence bias would vary as a function of age such that negative affect might have a stronger impact on valence bias in older age when a more positive bias might otherwise be expected. In contrast, we did not have predictions about the effects of social connectedness as a function of age. Thus, we added the interaction between negative affect and age as a predictor of valence bias. Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study The modification of attentional bias to emotional information: A review of the techniques, mechanisms, and relevance to emotional disorders Neural substrates of increased memory sensitivity for negative stimuli in major depression Cognitive Therapy and the Emotional Disorders Neural biases to covert and overt signals of fear: Dissociation by trait anxiety and depression Encyclopedia of Human Behavior 2nd edn An 'integrative neuroscience' platform: Application to profiles of negativity and positivity bias Negativity bias' in risk for depression and anxiety: Brain-body fear circuitry correlates, 5-HTT-LPR and early life stress Altered emotional interference processing in affective and cognitive-control brain circuitry in major depression Emotion regulation and aging Association between a directly translated cognitive measure of negative bias and self-reported psychiatric symptoms Amygdala and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activation to masked angry faces in children and adolescents with generalized anxiety disorder Facial emotion processing in major depression: A systematic review of neuroimaging findings Brain-behavior relationships in the experience and regulation of negative emotion in healthy children: Implications for risk for childhood depression Twice the negativity bias and half the positivity offset: Evaluative responses to emotional information in depression Cognitive approaches to emotion and emotional disorders Cognitive bias modification for interpretation in major depression: Effects on memory and stress reactivity Resolution of lexical ambiguity by emotional state Seeing the world through non rose-colored glasses: Anxiety and the amygdala response to blended expressions Anxiety-related bias in the classification of emotionally ambiguous facial expressions Inverse amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex responses to surprised faces Neural responses to ambiguity involve domain-general and domain-specific emotion processing systems Corrugator muscle responses are associated with individual differences in positivity-negativity bias Resolving uncertainty in a social world Uncertainty in decision making in medicine: A scoping review and thematic analysis of conceptual models Risky business: The neuroeconomics of decision making under uncertainty Economic Uncertainty Before and During the Covid-19 Pandemic (No. w27418 Handling uncertainty and ambiguity in the COVID-19 pandemic Getting to know you: Reputation and trust in two-person economic exchange How do you see me? The neural basis of motivated meta-perception Positivity bias in judging ingroup members' emotional expressions Inaccurate group meta-perceptions drive negative out-group attributions in competitive contexts Amygdala response to happy faces as a function of extraversion Temperament, development, and personality Influence of extraversion and neuroticism on subjective well-being: Happy and unhappy people The devil you know: Neuroticism predicts neural response to uncertainty Negativity bias in false memory: Moderation by neuroticism after a delay Personality and cognitive processing of affective information The relationship between the five-factor model of personality and symptoms of clinical disorders: A meta-analysis The relation of neuroticism and extraversion to symptoms of anxiety and depression in the general population Trait anxiety is associated with negative interpretations when resolving valence ambiguity of surprised faces Don't like what you see? Give it time: Longer reaction times associated with increased positive affect Valence resolution of ambiguous facial expressions using an emotional oddball task The primacy of negative interpretations when resolving the valence of ambiguous facial expressions The dynamic process of ambiguous emotion perception Individual differences in valence bias: fMRI evidence of the initial negativity hypothesis Social modulation of stress responses Loving-kindness meditation increases social connectedness The contours of positive human health Reinterpreting the empathy-altruism relationship: When one into one equals oneness Upward spirals of positive emotions counter downward spirals of negativity: Insights from the broaden-andbuild theory and affective neuroscience on the treatment of emotion dysfunctions and deficits in psychopathology Loneliness is a unique predictor of age-related differences in systolic blood pressure The relationship between social connectedness and anxiety, self-esteem, and social identity Don't hide your happiness! Positive emotion dissociation, social connectedness, and psychological functioning Cardiovascular intraindividual variability in later life: The influence of social connectedness and positive emotions Estimating and interpreting latent variable interactions: A tutorial for applying the latent moderated structural equations method Age-related patterns in social networks among European Americans and African Americans: Implications for socioemotional selectivity across the life span A. negativity bias for ambiguous facial-expression valence during childhood: Converging evidence from behavior and facial corrugator muscle responses Positive valence bias and parent-child relationship security moderate the association between early institutional caregiving and internalizing symptoms Exploring valence bias as a metric for fronto-amygdalar connectivity and depressive symptoms in childhood Adult age differences in the interpretation of surprised facial expressions Positivity effect in aging: Evidence for the primacy of positive responses toward emotional ambiguity Daily emotion links task-based negativity bias and variability in depressive symptoms Dispositional negativity: An integrative psychological and neurobiological perspective Functional connectivity of the amygdala in early-childhood-onset depression Spring break or heart break? Extending valence bias to emotional words The neural architecture of emotion regulation Cortisol response enhance negative valence perception for ambiguous facial expressions One step at a time: Physical activity is linked to positive interpretations of ambiguity Propensity to reappraise promotes resilience to stress-induced negativity bias The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants The Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces (KDEF) (CD ROM from Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology section The Umeå university database of facial expressions: A validation study International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Affective raTings of Pictures and instruction Manual Mousetracker: Software for studying real-time mental processing using a computer mouse-tracking method Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale Targeting emotion dysregulation in the treatment of self-injury NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) 3 (Psychological Assessment Resources Internal consistency, retest reliability, and their implications for personality scale validity State-trait anxiety inventory for adults The empathy and systemizing quotient: The psychometric properties of the Dutch version and a review of the cross-cultural stability The empathy quotient: An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences Interpersonal emotion regulation: Implications for affiliation, perceived support, relationships, and well-being Mplus User's Guide 7th edn Applied Missing Data Analysis The present approach. In the present study, we set out to (a) determine the putative sources of variability that might distinguish individuals with a more negative versus positive valence bias, and (b) establish the importance of the valence bias task in exploring individual differences relevant to subclinical symptomology. We conducted an analysis of data collected across 14 experiments that similarly assessed valence bias in each participant, and also collected a variety of individual difference measures thought to have a potential role in explaining valence bias. Our hypothesis is that valence bias is related to (some set of) individual differences in measures of negative affectivity and social connectedness. We predict that the effect of social connectedness will be particularly important in promoting a positive valence bias given that, at least in young adults, the negative bias represents the initial or default response. In other words, although there are likely important individual differences that reinforce a more negative bias, the ability to override the default negativity may more critically rely on variability in social connectedness that helps to downregulate or overcome the tendency toward negativity. In contrast, a low propensity toward experiencing negative emotions (low sensitivity to distressing stimuli) may not be sufficient to demonstrate a positive bias, at least in young adulthood. This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (NIMH111640; PI: Neta), and by Nebraska Tobacco Settlement Biomedical Research Enhancement Funds. We thank Nicholas R. Harp for help with scoring, and Kayla Clark for help with compiling data. The authors declare no competing interests. The online version contains supplementary material available at https ://doi. org/10.1038/s4159 8-020-80471 -2.Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to M.N.Reprints and permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints.Publisher's note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creat iveco mmons .org/licen ses/by/4.0/.