key: cord-0005394-1i1rhwcw authors: Shi, Kan; Lu, Jiafang; Fan, Hongxia; Jia, Jianming; Song, Zhaoli; Li, Wendong; Gao, Jing; Chen, Xuefeng; Hu, Weipeng title: Rationality of 17 cities’ public perception of SARS and predictive model of psychological behavior date: 2003 journal: Chin Sci Bull DOI: 10.1007/bf03184166 sha: 394e84e76a81b298470774fb2b3eb198c410bdb0 doc_id: 5394 cord_uid: 1i1rhwcw This study investigated the feature of Chinese peoples’ perception of SARS by surveying a stratified sample of 4231 people from 17 cities in China, and primarily proposed a risk perception centered predictive model of psychological behavior in crisis. The results indicated that, negative SARS-related information, especially information of personal interest, will arouse people’s risk perception of high level, and lead to irrational nervousness or scare; but positive SARS-related information, including recovery information and that with measures taken by government, can decrease the level of risk perception. In the middle of May, people felt the highest level of risk on the SARS pathogens; the following are the physical health condition and infectivity after recovering from SARS; they are factors that need special attention. SEM result analyses supported our hypotheses in that SARS-related information affect people’s coping behavior and mental health through their risk perception, the four indices of risk assessment, feeling of nervousness, coping behavior and mental health are effective presentimental indices for public psychological behavior in risky events. Generally speaking, humans are assumed to be rational when perceiving risky event, judging and adopting behavior, however, Herbert. Simon, the Nobel Laureate in economic science of 1987, claimed that humans have bounded rationality due to fundamental limitations in mman mental processes, so they do not think rationally in real activities [l] ; and the psychologist Kahnemen won the international recognition again in 2002 for having integrated insights from psychological research into economic science, especially concerning human judgment and decision-making under uncertainty. He premises that cognitive limitations cause people to employ various simplifying strategies and rules of thumb to ease the burden of mentally processing information to make judgments and decisions. These simple rules of thumb, including availability heuristic, representativeness heuristic and anchoring! adjustment heuristic, are often useful in helping us deal with complexity and ambiguity. Under many circumstances, however, they lead to predictably faulty judgments known as cognitive biases [2] . Slovic also pointed out the similar bias in human perception of risky event [3] . But there are rarely relevant researches in the method of large-size survey in real situation, especially in the background of eastern culture and a real social threatening risky event. Since November 2002, the infectious disease of Severe Acute Respiratory Symptoms (SARS) has affected Guangdong, Hongkong, Beijing and North China. Due to its nature of strong infectivity and deadliness and since no definite preventive and treating solution has yet to be found, the crisis confronted Chinese society with a tremendous challenge. In protecting against SARS and saving SARS patients, the Chinese government instituted a variety of valid measures to control the spreading sources and preventing people from being infected, which brought the infectious disease under control in a relatively short period and gained universal positive recognition. So, in this risky event of SARS, what are the characteristics of Chinese people's risk perception? Particularly, in a real risky event, is the rationality of the public consistent with the results found in western culture? What are the effects of various information on people's rational mental processing? And how does the perception buffer the information's effects on public coping behavior and mental health? This should be meaningful for exploring the risk perception rules, especially for addressing the rationality in the background of a real risky event. When confronted with a crisis, people are possibly afraid or scared if there is neither objective information nor a definitive solution. So risk communication becomes critically important. Risk communication is a social process; its aim is to keep people informed of a crisis, on which people generate appropriate coping behavior and are involved in risk decisions. Risk communication usually happens when human risk perception arises, and functions through depressing people's risk perception [4] . Trauma [5] suggested that risk communication is a systematic process; its key point is the risk assessment of the public and safety educational management to disaster. Since the end of April, the Ministry of Health, the Peo-pIe's Republic of China, began to update the news of the nation-wide infectious situation everyday, by dint of numbers of new patients and possible patients in each province, assuring people informed of what happened with SARS in time. This action is a good example of risk communication conducted by the Chinese government over recent years. Besides the exploration for rationality illustrated above, in order to examine the communication results on SARS between the government and the public, we should know, in the campaign of the Chinese government successfully protecting against and controlling SARS, what are the impacts of the risk communication between the government and the public? What kind of role, from the psychological sense, did the risk communication play in protecting against SARS; and how did the public react to the never-met infectious disease and to governmental risk communication? The addressing of these questions is valuable for establishing a predictive system of social-psychological behavior in crisis [7] . These two aspects are the focuses of interest in this study. ( i ) Research purposes. This study is conducted in the background of SARS threat and SARS-related information stimulus, and the researchers investigated diversified groups of people from variety of areas in China with different level of SARS infection, aiming at discovering features of individuals' risk perceptions, and their effects on public coping behavior, mental health and so forth. In order to explore people's risk perception features and their impact on such proposed presentimental indices as peo-pIe's coping behavior and mental health, and to examine how the informational factors influence risk perception, we established a risk perception centered predictive model of psychological behavior through empirical research. The results can provide psychological and managerial suggestion in protecting against SARS, and will also lay the theoretical foundation for the nation to establish psychological presentimental system against crisis. ( ii) Hypothesized structural model. Risk is the possible occurrence of an unwanted or dangerous event in an uncertain situation. Risk perceptions are individuals' subjective experiences and perceptions of external risks. These perceptions mayor not be consistent with reality. Risk perceptions emphasize individuals' experience through intuitive judgment and subjective feeling, which are affected by p~chological, social, situational and cultural elements [3,6, ] . Slovic [3] proposed the psychometric model of risk perception, public perceptual feature of SARS could also be represented through referring to this psychometric model. We proposed a predictive model from the following three aspects (Fig. 1) . (l) Positive information and negative information. As the independent variables in the predictive model, according to its nature in threatening or protecting individual safety, the information could be divided into positive information and negative information. In combination with the information issued by the Ministry of Health and prevalent mass media, we categorized them into two groups: SARS-related infected information are negative information (suggesting negative consequences), i.e. the number of new SARS cases; recovery information and that with measures taken by government to prevent against SARS are positive information (suggesting positive consequences), i.e. new recovery cases and the measures taken by government. (2) Risk perception. Slovic [3] proposed the psychometric model of risk perception and concluded its important dimensions and features; he claimed that people assess all kinds of risky events mainly from the perspective of" controllability" and" familiarity", whose higher end of level is perceived as "uncontrollable" and "unknown". In the quadrant composed of the two factors, every risky event is relatively located at a point, which could directly exhibit human perceptual feature of the risk. (3) Psychological behavior. In current study, we considered the 6 indices, including risk assessment, feeling of nervousness, coping behavior, mental health, SARS situation anticipation and economy development anticipation. (iii) Hypotheses Hypothesis 1. In the crisis of SARS, SARS-related negative information will accelerate individual risk perception, and lead to public irrational nervousness or scare; but positive information, especially that with measures taken by government have significant impact in depressing public irrational cognition. Hypothesis 2. The unknown and uncontrollable elements of SARS per se are the key factors inducing public feeling ofunsafely. Hypothesis 3. SARS-related information will influence public psychological behavior through their risk perception, and the 6 variables of risk assessment, feeling of nervousness, coping behavior, mental health, SARS situation anticipation and economy development anticipation are effective presentimental indices in predicting public psychological behavior in crisis. ( i ) Time range. The investigation was conducted in the period of May 9 to 19, 2003; both the questionnaire distribution and feedback were finished in the range. Since the first time official report on April 21, the SARS situation is in the stage of gradually being controlled, but SARS still threats. In the later division of SARS infectious districts, we determine the infectious degree of the districts mainly according to the new SARS cases/possi- Factor 2 is "Recovery information", including new recovery cases and cumulative cases of leaving hospital, in the nature of positive SARS-related information. Factor 3 is "information of personal interest", including if there are cases in their organization or living area; if there are cases in people they know; if there are cases in their age. They are all in the nature of negative SARS-related information. Factor 4 is "information with measures taken by government" , including government official speech, news press, the blocking ways against SARS spreading, the improvement of hospital treatment and conditions, buswater-electricity supply, in the nature of positive SARSrelated information. Based on the factor analysis, we divide the 17 cities into 5 groups: Beijing is the most severe infectious district; Tianjin, Shijiazhuang, Taiyuan and Hohhot are severe infectious districts; Xi' an, Shenyang, Hangzhou, Ningbo, Shanghai, Wuxi, Nanchang, Wuhan, Changsha, Chongqing are slightly infectious districts; Guangzhou is infection recession district; Guiyang is the district without infection. Because the sample distribution among different districts are not equal, we choose one city from each group and explore, among districts with differing infectious situation, people's risk assessment to the abovementioned 4 types of information. Chosen cities are: Beijing (the most severe infectious district), I-bhhot (severe infectious district), Wuhan (slightly severe district), Guiyang (district without infection), Guangzhou (infection recession district). ble cases when we collected the data, so our division of the infectious districts would not be mixed. ( ii) Measurement. The questionnaire consists of three parts, and each is described below. (l) SARS-related information. It represented two groups of items: information concerning with SARS per se, including its characteristic, infectivity, death rate, etc.; and information with preventive measures, including official speech, protecting and insulations against SARS, public buses, supermarket supply, etc. There were 23 items in all, and all used 5-point Likert scale. (2) Risk perception scale. It was derived from the psychometric model of risk perception proposed by Slovic [3] ; we combined it with 6 events of SARS: SARS pathogen, spreading and infectivity, recovery rate, preventive measures, infectivity after recovery, and aftereffects on physical health. The questionnaire asked how familiar and controllable the respondents felt about on the 6 events, and their overall feeling respectively on these two dimensions. All questions took the form of 5-point Likert scale. (3) Social-psychological presentimental indices. They consist of 6 social-psychological presentimental indices: risk perception, feeling of nervousness, SARS situation anticipation, mental health, coping behavior and economy development anticipation. The mental health scale is the General Health Questionnaire 12 [8] , and adopts 5-point Likert scale, the rest of the presentimental indices use 1O-point scale. Before May 9, through printed questionnaire or web pages, we obtained 236 respondents from Beijing, and conducted a pilot study on the designed questionnaire. On analysis of the pilot study results, we deleted or modified items which are hard to understand or with low reliability, and formed the questionnaire ofthis study.• (iii) Sample. Questionnaires were issued to 17 cities by electronically mailing to colleagues, who then printed the forms locally. The removal of questionnaires with too many missing or extreme values resulted in 4231 valid subjects. Demographical characteristics of respondents are listed in Table 1 . ( i ) Analysis on informational factors influencing risk perception. We performed factor analysis using the statistical software of SPSS 11.5 on the 23 items which measure the importance of each information in people's judging of risk. Using Varimax rotation, we obtained four factors, with a cumulative squared loading of 62.27% after deleting three items which loaded too low. We performed the factor analysis again, resulting in a clearer 4-factor structure, with the cumulative squared loading rising to 65.69%. Later analysis accepted the 4-factor model, with 20 items in all. Factor 1 is "SARS infected information" , including Variance analysis on the risk assessment to the information among the five differentially infected districts indicated that, the effects of all the four types information are significantly different (Information of being infected: F 4 ,1191 = 7.154, P