DOI: 10.1126/science.1199644 , 176 (2011);331 Science , et al.Jean-Baptiste Michel Quantitative Analysis of Culture Using Millions of Digitized Books This copy is for your personal, non-commercial use only. clicking here.colleagues, clients, or customers by , you can order high-quality copies for yourIf you wish to distribute this article to others here.following the guidelines can be obtained byPermission to republish or repurpose articles or portions of articles ): January 14, 2011 www.sciencemag.org (this infomation is current as of The following resources related to this article are available online at http://www.sciencemag.org/content/331/6014/176.full.html version of this article at: including high-resolution figures, can be found in the onlineUpdated information and services, http://www.sciencemag.org/content/suppl/2010/12/16/science.1199644.DC1.html can be found at: Supporting Online Material http://www.sciencemag.org/content/331/6014/176.full.html#related found at: can berelated to this article A list of selected additional articles on the Science Web sites http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/collection/sociology Sociology subject collections:This article appears in the following registered trademark of AAAS. is aScience2011 by the American Association for the Advancement of Science; all rights reserved. The title CopyrightAmerican Association for the Advancement of Science, 1200 New York Avenue NW, Washington, DC 20005. (print ISSN 0036-8075; online ISSN 1095-9203) is published weekly, except the last week in December, by theScience o n J a n u a ry 1 4 , 2 0 1 1 w w w .s ci e n ce m a g .o rg D o w n lo a d e d f ro m http://www.sciencemag.org/about/permissions.dtl http://www.sciencemag.org/about/permissions.dtl http://www.sciencemag.org/content/331/6014/176.full.html http://www.sciencemag.org/content/suppl/2010/12/16/science.1199644.DC1.html http://www.sciencemag.org/content/331/6014/176.full.html#related http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/collection/sociology http://www.sciencemag.org/ Quantitative Analysis of Culture Using Millions of Digitized Books Jean-Baptiste Michel,1,2,3,4,5*† Yuan Kui Shen,2,6,7 Aviva Presser Aiden,2,6,8 Adrian Veres,2,6,9 Matthew K. Gray,10 The Google Books Team,10 Joseph P. Pickett,11 Dale Hoiberg,12 Dan Clancy,10 Peter Norvig,10 Jon Orwant,10 Steven Pinker,5 Martin A. Nowak,1,13,14 Erez Lieberman Aiden1,2,6,14,15,16,17*† We constructed a corpus of digitized texts containing about 4% of all books ever printed. Analysis of this corpus enables us to investigate cultural trends quantitatively. We survey the vast terrain of ‘culturomics,’ focusing on linguistic and cultural phenomena that were reflected in the English language between 1800 and 2000. We show how this approach can provide insights about fields as diverse as lexicography, the evolution of grammar, collective memory, the adoption of technology, the pursuit of fame, censorship, and historical epidemiology. Culturomics extends the boundaries of rigorous quantitative inquiry to a wide array of new phenomena spanning the social sciences and the humanities. R eading small collections of carefully cho- sen works enables scholars to make pow- erful inferences about trends in human thought. However, this approach rarely enables precise measurement of the underlying phenome- na. Attempts to introduce quantitative methods into the study of culture (1–6) have been ham- pered by the lack of suitable data. We report the creation of a corpus of 5,195,769 digitized books containing ~4% of all books ever published. Computational analysis of this corpus enables us to observe cultural trends and subject them to quantitative investigation. ‘Culturomics’ extends the boundaries of scientific inquiry to a wide array of new phenomena. The corpus has emerged from Google’s effort to digitize books. Most books were drawn from over 40 university libraries around the world. Each page was scanned with custom equipment (7), and the text was digitized by means of optical character recognition (OCR). Additional vol- umes, both physical and digital, were contributed by publishers. Metadata describing the date and place of publication were provided by the li- braries and publishers and supplemented with bibliographic databases. Over 15 million books have been digitized [~12% of all books ever published (7)]. We selected a subset of over 5 million books for analysis on the basis of the quality of their OCR and metadata (Fig. 1A and fig. S1) (7). Periodicals were excluded. The resulting corpus contains over 500 billion words, in English (361 billion), French (45 billion), Spanish (45 billion), German (37 billion), Chinese (13 billion), Russian (35 billion), and Hebrew (2 billion). The oldest works were published in the 1500s. The early decades are represented by only a few books per year, comprising several hundred thousand words. By 1800, the corpus grows to 98 million words per year; by 1900, 1.8 billion; and by 2000, 11 billion (fig. S2). The corpus cannot be read by a human. If you tried to read only English-language entries from the year 2000 alone, at the reasonable pace of 200 words/min, without interruptions for food or sleep, it would take 80 years. The sequence of letters is 1000 times longer than the human genome: If you wrote it out in a straight line, it would reach to the Moon and back 10 times over (8). To make release of the data possible in light of copyright constraints, we restricted this initial study to the question of how often a given 1-gram or n-gram was used over time. A 1-gram is a string of characters uninterrupted by a space; this in- cludes words (“banana”, “SCUBA”) but also num- bers (“3.14159”) and typos (“excesss”). An n-gram is a sequence of 1-grams, such as the phrases “stock market” (a 2-gram) and “the United States of America” (a 5-gram). We restricted n to 5 and lim- ited our study to n-grams occurring at least 40 times in the corpus. Usage frequency is computed by dividing the number of instances of the n-gram in a given year by the total number of words in the corpus in that year. For instance, in 1861, the 1-gram “slavery” appeared in the corpus 21,460 times, on 11,687 pages of 1208 books. The corpus contains 386,434,758 words from 1861; thus, the frequency is 5.5 × 10−5. The use of “slavery” peaked during the Civil War (early 1860s) and then again during the civil rights movement (1955–1968) (Fig. 1B) In contrast, we compare the frequency of “the Great War” to the frequencies of “World War I” and “World War II”. References to “the Great War” peak between 1915 and 1941. But although its frequency drops thereafter, interest in the un- derlying events had not disappeared; instead, they are referred to as “World War I” (Fig. 1C). These examples highlight two central factors thatcontributetoculturomictrends.Culturalchange guides the concepts we discuss (such as “slavery”). Linguistic change, which, of course, has cultural roots, affects the words we use for those concepts (“the Great War” versus “World War I”). In this paper, we examine both linguistic changes, such as changes in the lexicon and grammar, and cul- tural phenomena, such as how we remember peo- ple and events. The full data set, which comprises over two billion culturomic trajectories, is available for download or exploration at www.culturomics.org and ngrams.googlelabs.com. The size of the English lexicon. How many words are in the English language (9)? We call a 1-gram “common” if its frequency is greater than one per billion. [This corresponds to the frequency of the words listed in leading dic- tionaries (7) (fig. S3).] We compiled a list of all common 1-grams in 1900, 1950, and 2000, based on the frequency of each 1-gram in the preced- ing decade. These lists contained 1,117,997 com- mon 1-grams in 1900, 1,102,920 in 1950, and 1,489,337 in 2000. Not all common 1-grams are English words. Many fell into three nonword categories: (i) 1-grams with nonalphabetic characters (“l8r”, “3.14159”), (ii) misspellings (“becuase”, “abberation”), and (iii) foreign words (“sensitivo”). To estimate the number of English words, we manually annotated random samples from the lists of common 1-grams (7) and determined what fraction were members of the above nonword categories. The result ranged from 51% of all common 1-grams in 1900 to 31% in 2000. Using this technique, we estimated the num- ber of words in the English lexicon as 544,000 in 1900, 597,000 in 1950, and 1,022,000 in 2000. The lexicon is enjoying a period of enormous growth: The addition of ~8500 words/year has increased the size of the language by over 70% during the past 50 years (Fig. 2A). Notably, we found more words than appear in any dictionary. For instance, the 2002 Webster’s Third New International Dictionary (W3), which keeps track of the contemporary American lexicon, lists approximately 348,000 single-word wordforms (10); the American Heritage Dictionary of the En- glish Language, Fourth Edition (AHD4) lists 116,161 (11). (Both contain additional multiword entries.) Part of this gap is because dictionaries often RESEARCH ARTICLE 1Program for Evolutionary Dynamics, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA. 2Cultural Observatory, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA. 3Institute for Quantitative Social Sciences, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA. 4Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA. 5Department of Systems Biology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02115, USA. 6Laboratory-at-Large, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA. 7Computer Science and Artificial Intelligence Laboratory, Massachusetts Institute of Technology (MIT), Cambridge, MA 02139, USA. 8Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, 02115, USA. 9Harvard College, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA. 10Google, Mountain View, CA 94043, USA. 11Houghton Mifflin Harcourt, Boston, MA 02116, USA. 12Encyclopaedia Britannica, Chicago, IL 60654, USA. 13Department of Organismic and Evolutionary Biology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA. 14Department of Mathematics, Harvard Uni- versity, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA. 15Broad Institute of Harvard and MIT, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA. 16School of Engineering and Applied Sciences, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA. 17Harvard Society of Fellows, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA. *These authors contributed equally to this work. †To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: jb.michel@gmail.com (J.-B.M.); erez@erez.com (E.L.A.) 14 JANUARY 2011 VOL 331 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org176 o n J a n u a ry 1 4 , 2 0 1 1 w w w .s ci e n ce m a g .o rg D o w n lo a d e d f ro m http://www.sciencemag.org/ exclude proper nouns (fig. S4) and compound words (“whalewatching”). Even accounting for these factors,we found many undocumented words, such as “aridification” (the process by which a geo- graphic region becomes dry), “slenthem” (a musical instrument), and, appropriately, the word “deletable.” This gap between dictionaries and the lexicon results from a balance that every dictionary must strike: It must be comprehensive enough to be a useful reference but concise enough to be printed, shipped, and used. As such, many infrequent words are omitted. To gauge how well dictio- naries reflect the lexicon, we ordered our year-2000 lexicon by frequency, divided it into eight deciles (ranging from 10−9 to 10−8, to 10−2 to 10−1) and sampled each decile (7). We manually checked how many sample words were listed in the Oxford English Dictionary (OED) (12) and in the Merriam-Webster Unabridged Dictionary(MWD). (We excluded proper nouns, because neither the OED nor MWD lists them.) Both dictionaries had excellent coverage of high-frequency words but less coverage for frequencies below 10−6: 67% of words in the 10−9 to 10−8 range were listed in neither dictionary (Fig. 2B). Consistent with Zipf’s famous law, a large fraction of the words in our lexicon (63%) were in this lowest- frequency bin. As a result, we estimated that 52% of the English lexicon—the majority of the words used in English books—consists of lexical “dark matter” undocumented in standard references (12). To keep up with the lexicon, dictionaries are updated regularly (13). We examined how well these changes corresponded with changes in ac- tual usage by studying the 2077 1-gram headwords added to AHD4 in 2000. The overall frequency of these words, such as “buckyball” and “netiquette”, has soared since 1950: Two-thirds exhibited recent sharp increases in frequency (>2× from 1950 to 2000) (Fig. 2C). Nevertheless, there was a lag be- tween lexicographers and the lexicon. Over half the words added to AHD4 were part of the English lexicon a century ago (frequency >10−9 from 1890 to 1900). In fact, some newly added words, such as “gypseous” and “amplidyne”, have already un- dergone a steep decline in frequency (Fig. 2D). Not only must lexicographers avoid adding words that have fallen out of fashion, they must also weed obsolete words from earlier editions. This is an imperfect process. We found 2220 ob- solete 1-gram headwords (“diestock”, “alkales- cent”) in AHD4. Their mean frequency declined throughout the 20th century and dipped below 10−9 decades ago (Fig. 2D, inset). Our results suggest that culturomic tools will aid lexicographers in at least two ways: (i) find- ing low-frequency words that they do not list, and (ii) providing accurate estimates of current fre- quency trends to reduce the lag between changes in the lexicon and changes in the dictionary. The evolution of grammar. Next, we exam- ined grammatical trends. We studied the English irregular verbs, a classic model of grammatical change (14–17). Unlike regular verbs, whose past tense is generated by adding -ed (jump/jumped), irregular verbs are conjugated idiosyncratically (stick/stuck, come/came, get/got) (15). All irregular verbs coexist with regular com- petitors (e.g., “strived” and “strove”) that threaten to supplant them (Fig. 2E and fig. S5). High- frequency irregulars, which are more readily remembered, hold their ground better. For in- stance, we found “found” (frequency: 5 × 10−4) 200,000 times more often than we finded “finded.” In contrast, “dwelt” (frequency: 1 × 10−5) dwelt in our data only 60 times as often as “dwelled” dwelled. We defined a verb’s “regularity” as the percentage of instances in the past tense (i.e., the sum of “drived”, “drove”, and “driven”) in which the regular form is used. Most irregulars have been stable for the past 200 years, but 16% underwent a change in regularity of 10% or more (Fig. 2F). These changes occurred slowly: It took 200 years for our fastest-moving verb (“chide”) to go from 10% to 90%. Otherwise, each trajectory was sui generis; we observed no characteristic shape. For instance, a few verbs, such as “spill”, regularized at a constant speed, but others, such as “thrive” and “dig”, transitioned in fits and starts (7). In some cases, the trajectory suggested a rea- son forthetrend.For example,with “sped/speeded” the shift in meaning from “to move rapidly” and toward “to exceed the legal limit” appears to have been the driving cause (Fig. 2G). Six verbs (burn, chide, smell, spell, spill, and thrive) regularized between 1800 and 2000 (Fig. 2F). Four are remnants of a now-defunct phono- logical process that used -t instead of -ed; they are members of a pack of irregulars that survived by virtue of similarity (bend/bent, build/built, burn/ burnt, learn/learnt, lend/lent, rend/rent, send/sent, smell/smelt, spell/spelt, spill/spilt, and spoil/spoilt). Verbs have been defecting from this coalition for centuries (wend/went, pen/pent, gird/girt, geld/ gelt, and gild/gilt all blend/blent into the domi- nant -ed rule). Culturomic analysis reveals that the collapse of this alliance has been the most significant driver of regularization in the past 200 years. The regularization of burnt, smelt, spelt, and spilt originated in the United States; the forms still cling to life in British English (Fig. 2, E and F). But the -t irregulars may be doomed in England too. Each year, a population the size of Cambridge adopts “burned” in lieu of “burnt”. Fig.1.Culturomic analy- sesstudymillionsofbooks at once. (A) Top row: Au- thors have been writing for millennia; ~129 mil- lion book editions have been published since the adventoftheprintingpress (upper left). Second row: Libraries and publishing houses provide books to Googleforscanning(mid- dle left). Over 15 million bookshavebeendigitized. Third row: Each book is associatedwithmetadata. Fivemillionbooksarecho- senforcomputationalanal- ysis (bottom left). Bottom row:Aculturomictimeline shows the frequency of “apple” in English books over time (1800–2000). (B) Usage frequency of “slavery”. The Civil War (1861–1865) and the civil rights movement (1955–1968) are highlighted in red. The number in the upper left (1e-4 = 10–4) is the unit of frequency. (C) Usage frequency over time for “the Great War” (blue), “World War I” (green), and “World War II” (red). Frequency of the word "apple" Year 129 million books published 15 million books scanned 5 million books analyzed BA C www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 331 14 JANUARY 2011 177 RESEARCH ARTICLE o n J a n u a ry 1 4 , 2 0 1 1 w w w .s ci e n ce m a g .o rg D o w n lo a d e d f ro m http://www.sciencemag.org/ Although irregulars generally yield to regu- lars, two verbs did the opposite: light/lit and wake/woke. Both were irregular in Middle English, were mostly regular by 1800, and subsequently backtracked and are irregular again today. The fact that these verbs have been going back and forth for nearly 500 years highlights the gradual nature of the underlying process. Still, there was at least one instance of rapid progress by an irregular form. Presently, 1% of the English-speaking population switches from “sneaked” to “snuck” every year. Someone will have snuck off while you read this sentence. As before, this trend is more prominent in the United States but recently sneaked across the Atlantic: America is the world’s leading exporter of both regular and irregular verbs. Out with the old. Just as individuals forget the past (18, 19), so do societies (20) (fig. S6). To quantify this effect, we reasoned that the fre- quency of 1-grams such as “1951” could be used to measure interest in the events of the corre- sponding year, and we created plots for each year between 1875 and 1975. The plots had a characteristic shape. For example, “1951” was rarely discussed until the years immediately preceding 1951. Its fre- quency soared in 1951, remained high for 3 years, and then underwent a rapid decay, dropping by half over the next 15 years. Finally, the plots Fig. 2. Culturomics has profound consequences for the study of language, lexicography, and grammar. (A) The size of the English lexicon over time. Tick marks show the number of single words in three dictionaries (see text). (B) Fraction of words in the lexicon that appear in two different dictionaries as a function of usage frequency. (C) Five words added by the AHD in its 2000 update. Inset: Median fre- quency of new words added to AHD4 in 2000. The frequency of half of these words exceeded 10−9 as far back as 1890 (white dot). (D) Obsolete words added to AHD4 in 2000. Inset: Mean frequency of the 2220 AHD headwords whose current usage fre- quency is less than 10−9. (E) Usage frequency of irregular verbs (red) and their regular counterparts (blue). Some verbs (chide/chided) have regularized during the past two centuries. The trajectories for “speeded” and “speed up” (green) are similar, re- flecting the role of semantic factors in this instance of regularization. The verb “burn” first regularized in the United States (U.S. flag) and later in the United Kingdom (UK flag). The irregular “snuck” is rapidly gaining on “sneaked”. (F) Scatterplot of the irregular verbs; each verb’s position depends on its regularity (see text) in the early 19th century (x coor- dinate) and in the late 20th century (y coordinate). For 16% of the verbs, the change in regularity was greater than 10% (large font). Dashed lines sepa- rate irregular verbs (regularity < 50%) from reg- ular verbs (regularity > 50%). Six verbs became regular (upper left quadrant, blue), whereas two be- came irregular (lower right quadrant, red). Inset: The regularity of “chide” over time. (G) Median reg- ularity of verbs whose past tense is often signified with a -t suffix instead of -ed (burn, smell, spell, spill, dwell, learn, and spoil) in U.S. (black) and UK (gray) books. words added to AHD in 2000 0 2 4 x10-8 1800 2000 yc n e u q erf n ai d e M Decade rare words still in AHD 0 2 4 x10-9 yc n e u q erf n a e M 1800 2000 Decade AHD4 W3 OED A B C D E F G 14 JANUARY 2011 VOL 331 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org178 RESEARCH ARTICLE o n J a n u a ry 1 4 , 2 0 1 1 w w w .s ci e n ce m a g .o rg D o w n lo a d e d f ro m http://www.sciencemag.org/ enter a regime marked by slower forgetting: Collective memory has both a short-term and a long-term component. But there have been changes. The amplitude of the plots is rising every year: Precise dates are increasingly common. There is also a greater fo- cus on the present. For instance, “1880” declined to half its peak value in 1912, a lag of 32 years. In contrast, “1973” declined to half its peak by 1983, a lag of only 10 years. We are forgetting our past faster with each passing year (Fig. 3A). We were curious whether our increasing tendency to forget the old was accompanied by more rapid assimilation of the new (21). We di- vided a list of 147 inventions into time-resolved cohorts based on the 40-year interval in which they were first invented (1800–1840, 1840–1880, and 1880–1920) (7). We tracked the frequency of each invention in the nth year after it was invented as compared to its maximum value and plotted the median of these rescaled trajectories for each cohort. The inventions from the earliest cohort (1800–1840) took over 66 years from invention D o u b lin g t im e: 4 y rs Half life: 73 yrs Year of invention 0 5 x10-5 yc n e u q er F A B C D E F F re q u e n cy F re q u e n cy F re q u e n cy ( lo g ) M e d ia n f re q u e n cy (% o f p e a k va lu e ) M e d ia n f re q u e n cy M e d ia n f re q u e n cy ( lo g ) Fig. 3. Cultural turnover is accelerating. (A) We forget: frequency of “1883” (blue), “1910” (green), and “1950” (red). Inset: We forget faster. The half-life of the curves (gray dots) is getting shorter (gray line: moving average). (B) Cultural adoption is quicker. Median trajectory for three cohorts of inventions from three different time periods (1800–1840, blue; 1840–1880, green; 1880–1920, red). Inset: The telephone (green; date of invention, green arrow) and radio (blue; date of invention, blue arrow). (C) Fame of various personalities born between 1920 and 1930. (D) Frequency of the 50 most famous people born in 1871 (gray lines; median, thick dark gray line). Five examples are highlighted. (E) The median trajectory of the 1865 cohort is characterized by four parameters: (i) initial age of celebrity (34 years old, tick mark); (ii) doubling time of the subsequent rise to fame (4 years, blue line); (iii) age of peak celebrity (70 years after birth, tick mark), and (iv) half-life of the post-peak forgetting phase (73 years, red line). Inset: The doubling time and half-life over time. (F) The median trajectory of the 25 most famous personalities born between 1800 and 1920 in various careers. www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 331 14 JANUARY 2011 179 RESEARCH ARTICLE o n J a n u a ry 1 4 , 2 0 1 1 w w w .s ci e n ce m a g .o rg D o w n lo a d e d f ro m http://www.sciencemag.org/ to widespread impact (frequency >25% of peak). Since then, the cultural adoption of technology has become more rapid. The 1840–1880 invention cohort was widely adopted within 50 years; the 1880–1920 cohort within 27 (Fig. 3B and fig. S7). “In the future, everyone will be famous for 7.5 minutes” – Whatshisname. People, too, rise to prominence, only to be forgotten (22). Fame can be tracked by measuring the frequency of a person’s name (Fig. 3C). We compared the rise to fame of the most famous people of different eras. We took all 740,000 people with entries in Wikipedia, removed cases where several famous individuals share a name, and sorted the rest by birth date and frequency (23). For every year from 1800 to 1950, we constructed a cohort consisting of the 50 most famous people born in that year. For example, the 1882 cohort includes “Virginia Woolf” and “Felix Frankfurter”; the 1946 cohort includes “Bill Clinton” and “Steven Spielberg”. We plotted the median frequency for the names in each cohort over time (Fig. 3, D and E). The resulting trajectories were all similar. Each cohort had a pre-celebrity period (median frequency <10−9), followed by a rapid rise to prominence, a peak, and a slow de- cline. We therefore characterized each cohort using four parameters: (i) the age of initial celebrity, (ii) the doubling time of the initial rise, (iii) the age of peak celebrity, and (iv) the half-life of the decline (Fig. 3E). The age of peak celebrity has been con- sistent over time: about 75 years after birth. But the other parameters have been changing (fig. S8). Fame comes sooner and rises faster. Between the early 19th century and the mid-20th century, the age of initial celebrity declined from 43 to 29 years, and the doubling time fell from 8.1 to 3.3 years. As a result, the most famous people alive today are more famous—in books—than their predecessors. Yet this fame is increasingly short- lived: The post-peak half-life dropped from 120 to 71 years during the 19th century. We repeated this analysis with all 42,358 people in the databases of the Encyclopaedia Britannica (24), which reflect a process of expert curation that began in 1768. The results were similar (7) (fig. S9). Thus, people are getting more famous than ever before but are being forgotten more rapidly than ever. Fig. 4. Culturomics can be used to detectcensorship.(A)Usagefrequen- cy of “Marc Chagall” in German (red) as compared to English (blue). (B) Suppression of Leon Trotsky (blue), Grigory Zinoviev (green), and Lev Kamenev (red) in Russian texts, with noteworthy events indicated: Trotsky’s assassination (blue arrow), Zinoviev and Kamenev executed (red arrow), the Great Purge (red highlight), and perestroika (gray ar- row). (C) The 1976 and 1989 Tianan- men Square incidents both led to elevated discussion in English texts (scale shown on the right). Response to the 1989 incident is largely ab- sentinChinesetexts(blue,scaleshown on the left), suggesting government censorship. (D) While the Holly- wood Ten were blacklisted (red highlight) from U.S. movie studios, their fame declined (median: thick gray line). None of them were cred- ited in a film until 1960’s (aptly named) Exodus. (E) Artists and writ- ers in various disciplines were sup- pressed by the Nazi regime (red highlight). Incontrast,theNazisthem- selves (thick red line) exhibited a strong fame peak during the war years. (F) Distribution of suppres- sion indices for both English (blue) andGerman (red) for the period from 1933–1945. Three victims of Nazi suppression are highlighted at left (red arrows). Inset: Calculation of the suppression index for “Henri Matisse”. 天安門 F re q u e n cy F re q u e n cy F re q u e n cy F re q u e n cy A B C D E F 14 JANUARY 2011 VOL 331 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org180 RESEARCH ARTICLE o n J a n u a ry 1 4 , 2 0 1 1 w w w .s ci e n ce m a g .o rg D o w n lo a d e d f ro m http://www.sciencemag.org/ Occupational choices affect the rise to fame. We focused on the 25 most famous individuals born between 1800 and 1920 in seven occupations (ac- tors, artists, writers, politicians, biologists, phys- icists, and mathematicians), examining how their fame grewasa functionofage(Fig.3Fandfig.S10). Actors tend to become famous earliest, at around 30. But the fame of the actors we studied, whose ascent preceded the spread of television, rises slowly thereafter. (Their fame peaked at a frequency of 2 × 10−7.) The writers became fa- mous about a decade after the actors, but rose for longer and to a much higher peak (8 × 10−7). Politicians did not become famous until their 50s, when, upon being elected president of the United States (in 11 of 25 cases; 9 more were heads of other states), they rapidly rose to become the most famous of the groups (1 × 10−6). Science is a poor route to fame. Physicists and biologists eventually reached a similar level of fame as actors (1 × 10−7), but it took them far longer. Alas, even at their peak, mathematicians tend not to be appreciated by the public (2 × 10−8). Detecting censorship and suppression. Sup- pression of a person or an idea leaves quantifiable fingerprints (25). For instance, Nazi censorship of the Jewish artist Marc Chagall is evident by comparing the frequency of “Marc Chagall” in English and in German books (Fig. 4A). In both languages, there is a rapid ascent starting in the late 1910s (when Chagall was in his early 30s). In English, the ascent continues. But in German, the artist’s popularity decreases, reaching a nadir from 1936 to 1944, when his full name appears only once. (In contrast, from 1946 to 1954, “Marc Chagall” appears nearly 100 times in the German corpus.) Such examples are found in many coun- tries,includingRussia(Trotsky),China(Tiananmen Square), and the United States (the Hollywood Ten, blacklisted in 1947) (Fig. 4, B to D, and fig. S11). We probed the impact of censorship on a person’s cultural influence in Nazi Germany. Led by such figures as the librarian Wolfgang Hermann, the Nazis created lists of authors and artists whose “undesirable”, “degenerate” work was banned from libraries and museums and publicly burned (26–28). We plotted median usage in German for five such lists: artists (100 names) and writers of literature (147), politics (117), history (53), and philosophy (35) (Fig. 4E and fig. S12). We also included a collection of Nazi party members [547 names (7)]. The five suppressed groups exhibited a decline. This decline was modest for writers of history (9%) and literature (27%), but pronounced in politics (60%), philosophy (76%), and art (56%). The only group whose signal increased during the Third Reich was the Nazi party mem- bers [a 500% increase (7)]. Given such strong signals, we tested whether one could identify victims of Nazi repression de novo. We computed a “suppression index” (s) for each person by dividing their frequency from 1933 to 1945 by the mean frequency in 1925–1933 and in 1955–1965 (Fig. 4F, inset). In English, the distribution of suppression indices is tightly cen- tered around unity. Fewer than 1% of individuals lie at the extremes (s < 1/5 or s > 5). In German, the distribution is much wider, and skewed to the left: Suppression in Nazi Germany was not the exception, but the rule (Fig. 4F). At the far left, 9.8% of individuals showed strong suppression (s < 1/5). This population is highly enriched in documented victims of repression, such as Pablo Picasso (s = 0.12), the Bauhaus architect Walter Gropius (s = 0.16), and Hermann Maas (s < 0.01), an influential Protestant minister who helped many Jews flee (7). (Maas was later recognized by Israel’s Yad Vashem as one of the “Righteous Among the Nations.”) At the other extreme, 1.5% of the population exhibited a dra- matic rise (s > 5). This subpopulation is highly enriched in Nazis and Nazi-supporters, who bene- fited immensely from government propaganda (7). These results provide a strategy for rapidly identifying likely victims of censorship from a large pool of possibilities, and highlight how cul- turomic methods might complement existing his- torical approaches. Culturomics. Culturomics is the application of high-throughput data collection and analysis to the study of human culture. Books are a begin- ning, but we must also incorporate newspapers (29), manuscripts (30), maps (31), artwork (32), and a myriad of other human creations (33, 34). Of course, many voices—already lost to time— lie forever beyond our reach. Culturomic results are a new type of evidence in the humanities. As with fossils of ancient crea- tures, the challenge of culturomics lies in the in- terpretation of this evidence. Considerations of space restrict us to the briefest of surveys: a A B C D E F G H Fig. 5. Culturomics provides quantitative evidence for scholars in many fields. (A) Historical epi- demiology: “influenza” is shown in blue; the Russian, Spanish, and Asian flu epidemics are highlighted. (B) History of the Civil War. (C) Comparative history. (D) Gender studies. (E and F) History of science. (G) Historical gastronomy. (H) History of religion: “God”. www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 331 14 JANUARY 2011 181 RESEARCH ARTICLE o n J a n u a ry 1 4 , 2 0 1 1 w w w .s ci e n ce m a g .o rg D o w n lo a d e d f ro m http://www.sciencemag.org/ handful of trajectories and our initial interpreta- tions. Many more fossils (Fig. 5 and fig. S13), with shapes no less intriguing, beckon: (i) Peaks in “influenza” correspond with dates of known pandemics, suggesting the value of culturomic methods for historical epidemiol- ogy (35) (Fig. 5A and fig. S14). (ii) Trajectories for “the North”, “the South”, and finally “the enemy” reflect how polarization of the states preceded the descent into the Civil War (Fig. 5B). (iii) In the battle of the sexes, the “women” are gaining ground on the “men” (Fig. 5C). (iv) “féminisme” made early inroads in France, but the United States proved to be a more fertile environment in the long run (Fig. 5D). (v) “Galileo”, “Darwin”, and “Einstein” may be well-known scientists, but “Freud” is more deeply ingrained in our collective subconscious (Fig. 5E). (vi) Interest in “evolution” was waning when “DNA” came along (Fig. 5F). (vii) The history of the American diet offers many appetizing opportunities for future research; the menu includes “steak”, “sausage”, “ice cream”, “hamburger”, “pizza”, “pasta”, and “sushi” (Fig. 5G). (viii) “God” is not dead but needs a new publicist (Fig. 5H). These, together with the billions of other trajectories that accompany them, will furnish a great cache of bones from which to reconstruct the skeleton of a new science. References and Notes 1. E. O. Wilson, Consilience (Knopf, New York, 1998). 2. D. Sperber, Man (London) 20, 73 (1985). 3. S. Lieberson, J. Horwich, Sociol. Methodol. 38, 1 (2008). 4. L. L. Cavalli-Sforza, W. Marcus, X. Feldman, Cultural Transmission and Evolution (Princeton Univ. Press, Princeton, NJ, 1981). 5. P. Niyogi, The Computational Nature of Language Learning and Evolution (MIT, Cambridge, MA, 2006). 6. G. K. Zipf, The Psycho-biology of Language (Houghton Mifflin, Boston, 1935). 7. Materials and methods are available as supporting material on Science Online. 8. E. S. Lander et al.; International Human Genome Sequencing Consortium, Nature 409, 860 (2001). 9. A. W. Read, Am. Speech 8, 10 (1933). 10. Webster's Third New International Dictionary of the English Language, Unabridged, P. B. Gove, Ed. (Merriam-Webster, Springfield, MA, 1993). 11. The American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language, Fourth Edition, J. P. Pickett, Ed. (Houghton Mifflin, Boston, 2000). 12. Oxford English Dictionary, J. A. Simpson, E. S. C. Weiner, M. Proffitt, Eds. (Clarendon, Oxford, 1993). 13. J. Algeo, A. S. Algeo, Fifty Years among the New Words: A Dictionary of Neologisms, 1941–1991 (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, 1991). 14. S. Pinker, Words and Rules (Basic Books, New York, 1999). 15. Anthony S. Kroch, Language Variation Change 1, 199 (1989). 16. J. L. Bybee, Language 82, 711 (2006). 17. E. Lieberman, J. B. Michel, J. Jackson, T. Tang, M. A. Nowak, Nature 449, 713 (2007). 18. B. Milner, L. R. Squire, E. R. Kandel, Neuron 20, 445 (1998). 19. H. Ebbinghaus, Memory: A Contribution to Experimental Psychology (Dover Publications, New York, 1987). 20. M. Halbwachs, On Collective Memory, Lewis A. Coser, transl. (Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago, 1992). 21. S. Ulam, Bull. Am. Math. Soc. 64, 1 (1958). 22. L. Braudy, The Frenzy of Renown: Fame & Its History (Vintage Books, New York, 1997). 23. Wikipedia, 23 August 2010, www.wikipedia.org/. 24. Encyclopaedia Britannica, D. Hoiberg, Ed. (Encyclopaedia Britannica, Chicago, 2002). 25. Censorship: 500 Years of Conflict, V. Gregorian, Ed. (New York Public Library, New York, 1984). 26. W. Treß, Wider Den Undeutschen Geist: Bücherverbrennung 1933 (Parthas, Berlin, 2003). 27. G. Sauder, Die Bücherverbrennung: 10. Mai 1933 (Ullstein, Frankfurt am Main, Germany, 1985). 28. S. Barron, P. W. Guenther, Degenerate Art: The Fate of the Avant-garde in Nazi Germany (Los Angeles County Museum of Art, Los Angeles, 1991). 29. Google News Archive Search, http://news.google.com/ archivesearch. 30. Digital Scriptorium, www.scriptorium.columbia.edu. 31. Visual Eyes, www.viseyes.org. 32. ARTstor, www.artstor.org. 33. Europeana, www.europeana.eu. 34. Hathi Trust Digital Library, www.hathitrust.org. 35. J. M. Barry, The Great Influenza: The Epic Story of the Deadliest Plague in History (Viking Press, New York, 2004). 36. J.-B.M. was supported by the Foundational Questions in Evolutionary Biology Prize Fellowship and the Systems Biology Program (Harvard Medical School). Y.K.S. was supported by internships at Google. S.P. acknowledges support from NIH grant HD 18381. E.L.A. was supported by the Harvard Society of Fellows, the Fannie and John Hertz Foundation Graduate Fellowship, a National Defense Science and Engineering Graduate Fellowship, an NSF Graduate Fellowship, the National Space Biomedical Research Institute, and National Human Genome Research Institute grant T32 HG002295. This work was supported by a Google Research Award. The Program for Evolutionary Dynamics acknowledges support from the Templeton Foundation, NIH grant R01GM078986, and the Bill and Melinda Gates Foundation. Some of the methods described in this paper are covered by U.S. patents 7463772 and 7508978. We are grateful to D. Bloomberg, A. Popat, M. McCormick, T. Mitchison, U. Alon, S. Shieber, E. Lander, R. Nagpal, J. Fruchter, J. Guldi, J. Cauz, C. Cole, P. Bordalo, N. Christakis, C. Rosenberg, M. Liberman, J. Scheidlower, B. Zimmer, R. Darnton, and A. Spector for discussions; to C.-M. Hetrea and K. Sen for assistance with Encyclopaedia Britannica's database; to S. Eismann, W. Treß, and the City of Berlin Web site (berlin.de) for assistance in documenting victims of Nazi censorship; to C. Lazell and G. T. Fournier for assistance with annotation; to M. Lopez for assistance with Fig. 1; to G. Elbaz and W. Gilbert for reviewing an early draft; and to Google’s library partners and every author who has ever picked up a pen, for books. Supporting Online Material www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/science.1199644/DC1 Materials and Methods Figs. S1 to S19 References 27 October 2010; accepted 6 December 2010 Published online 16 December 2010; 10.1126/science.1199644 14 JANUARY 2011 VOL 331 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org182 RESEARCH ARTICLE o n J a n u a ry 1 4 , 2 0 1 1 w w w .s ci e n ce m a g .o rg D o w n lo a d e d f ro m http://www.sciencemag.org/