Quantifying and communicating peri‐operative risk Title Quantifying and communicating peri-operative risk Author(s) Irwin, MG; Kong, VKF Citation Anaesthesia (Oxford), 2014, v. 69 n. 12, p. 1299-1303 Issued Date 2014 URL http://hdl.handle.net/10722/215071 Rights Creative Commons: Attribution 3.0 Hong Kong License Editorial Quantifying and communicating peri-operative risk Shallow men believe in luck. Strong men believe in cause and effect —Ralph Waldo Emerson Life is risky Risk is the potential that a chosen action or activity (including the choice of inaction) will lead to a spe- cific outcome, and implies that the choice has an influence on the out- come. Most definitions are synony- mous with the possibility of an adverse event but, of course, a risk can also be taken in the hope of a favourable outcome, particularly with investment. There is also a personal perspective on risk. A fatalist person- ality may be very accepting and unconcerned about risk whereas more pragmatic individuals know that there can be modifiable factors involved. This can apply to health- care; for example, even though sur- gery may be necessary in a patient, there may be pharmaceutical inter- ventions that could reduce morbidity. Almost any human endeavour carries some risk. Staying in hospital is far riskier than travelling by aeroplane. A recent study showed that a one-night stay in hospital carries a 11.1% risk of nosocomial infection, a 3.4% risk of an adverse drug reaction related to human error or allergy, and a 0.4% risk of pressure ulcer due to immobil- isation [1]. In 2007 in the USA, there were 1.31 fatal crashes per 100 000 flight hours for non-commercial flights and 0.016 per 100 000 for major airlines [2]. Despite efforts to the contrary, healthcare is an intrinsi- cally hazardous business. Anaesthesia is a medical spe- cialty very much focused on risk management and patient safety and, consequently, the mortality risk attributable to anaesthesia itself has dropped dramatically over the years, from about one death in 1000 anaesthetic procedures in the 1940s to one in 100 000 in the early 2000s [3]. However, although anaesthesia is relatively safe, surgery can be very dangerous. In 2000, the 30-day mor- tality risk in the UK was one death in 34 emergency operations (2.9%) and 1:177 after elective surgery (0.6%) [4]. The European Surgical Outcomes Study was an observa- tional study in which data were col- lected on 46 539 patients aged ≥ 16 years undergoing non-cardiac surgery, over a seven-day period, in 498 hospitals across 28 European nations [5]. There was considerable variability from country to country but median death rates were 3% for elective and 10% for emergency sur- gery. Anaesthesia has an excellent track record for patient safety and has been described as the leading medical specialty in addressing such issues [6], yet it is apparent that the peri-operative process still has great potential for hazard from a host of factors, of which anaesthesia is but one. Ronald A. Howard, a pioneer of decision analysis, wanted to develop a scale that would more clearly confer risk rather than per- centages. He coined the term ‘microprobability’ to refer to an event with a chance of one in a million. From this concept, a ‘micro- mort’ (from ‘micro’ and ‘mortality’) is then a one in a million chance of death [7]. We face risk simply by being alive and this may be exacer- bated by indulging in various activi- ties. A mobile app is now available for illustrating how many micro- morts are involved in our daily activ- ities (see https://play.google.com/ store/apps/details?id=com.zanzibar- tech.micromorts). The use of micro- morts then allows us actually to quantify risk and translate it into whole numbers. A micromort denotes a one in a million chance of death from one-time dangerous events, a concept that can be easily understood and compared. A one in a million chance is, of course, rare but also an everyday occurrence. For example, the chance of a particular individual’s winning the weekly lottery is less than 1 in 32 million © 2014 The Association of Anaesthetists of Great Britain and Ireland 1299 Anaesthesia 2014, 69, 1299–1313 people, but conversely this jackpot gets won almost every week by somebody. Micromorts are an expression of acute risks, such that once that event has been completed the risk has gone. The risk of surgi- cal anaesthesia is in the range of var- ious day-to-day activities. Audits of the risk of death from a general anaesthetic alone vary considerably geographically, but may be around one death in 100 000 operations in a developed country [8], which equates to 10 micromorts per opera- tion. This is the same risk of death, on average, as riding a motorcycle for 60 miles or skydiving. Micro- morts rely on aggregated risk data for calculations, so their applicability to specific circumstances or individ- uals is limited. In contrast with mi- cromorts, there is also a unit called microlife which is a risk (or gain) representing a 30-minute change of life expectancy [9]. It is a way of mea- suring the impact of long-term hab- its on the human body. For example, smoking two cigarettes will ‘cost’ one microlife; whereas bonus life can be gained by taking a statin daily (one mi- crolife per day) or doing 20 minutes of moderate exercise daily (two microlives per day). A user-friendly microlife cal- culator (see http://journals.bmj.com/ site/microlives) is available. People in general are notoriously bad at calculat- ing risk. The concept of micromort and microlife can be useful for explaining various risks in our daily peri-operative practice to the general public. The blind leading the blind? Shared decision-making in the healthcare context very often depends on the understanding of numerical information, in either text or graphical format. The perception of harm and benefit associated with particular options is important for many health decisions. Surprisingly, not only patients but many doctors have severe problems mastering a host of numerical concepts that are prerequisites for understanding information about the harm and benefit of medical treatments [10]. Highly educated people can still have difficulty with relatively simple numeracy questions [11]. Numeracy influences the processing of both numerical and non-numerical infor- mation. Less numerate individuals are more susceptible to framing effects, more easily affected by non- numerical information such as mood states, and less sensitive to different levels of numerical risk [12]. The Berlin Numeracy Test is a new psy- chometric instrument for assessing statistical numeracy and risk literacy in an educated population [13]. It typically takes three minutes to com- plete and an online version is now also available (see http://www.risklit- eracy.org). Statisticians, clinicians and psy- chologists have recommended the use of numerical as opposed to ver- bal descriptions for risk communica- tion [14–16]. In addition to probability information, the way people perceive a risk message may be influenced by the framing of risk information, risk comparisons, the message’s qualitative content and trust [17]. Conveying relative risks alone without absolute risk or base- line risk is an example of non-trans- parent framing. Comparing benefits and harm using different scales, such as reporting benefits in big numbers by relative risk reduction and harm in small numbers by absolute risk increases, is another way of altering risk perception. An example is the 1995 contraceptive pill scare in the UK, where an alarming figure of a 100% increase (relative) in thrombo- sis caused by third-generation oral contraceptive pills was much more terrifying than a humble increment in absolute risk, from one in 7000 to two in 7000 [18]. Humans can be prone to unwittingly tricking them- selves with representative bias in risk assessment in gambling pursuits such as purchasing lottery tickets. If the odds of winning a lottery are one in a million, then buying two tickets will ‘double’ the chance to two in a million (an apparent 100% increase). However, the odds of not winning the jackpot by buying two lottery tickets hardly changes at all (from 99.9999% to 99.9998%; a change of 0.0001%). Non-transparent and mis- matched framings are common phenomena, even for scientific research published in leading medi- cal journals. Studies have revealed that up to half of articles report only relative risks or odds ratios, and about one third adopt mis- matched framing for risk-benefit discussion [19, 20]. Risk communi- cation with incomplete and mis- leading numerical descriptions hinders shared decision-making. Patients are likely to be familiar with the concept of risk, but human nature is such that many do not understand the relativity or perhaps even choose to ignore it. To use gambling again as an example, a recent $640 million lottery in the USA created much excitement and 1300 © 2014 The Association of Anaesthetists of Great Britain and Ireland Anaesthesia 2014, 69, 1299–1313 Editorial a scramble to buy tickets when the odds of winning were approxi- mately one in 175 million. That number may not mean much in itself, but in relative terms the chance of winning is 175 times less than that of being struck by light- ning in a given year, a fact that helps conceptualise probability. As in life, there is no zero risk and no certainty in any branch of medicine, but only risks that are more or less acceptable. Communi- cating risk information is important but, unfortunately, more difficult than might be expected. Patients’ values and preference are essential elements of shared decision-making. In 2011, a report on the peri-opera- tive care of surgical patients pub- lished by the National Confidential Enquiry into Patient Outcome and Death principally recommended that “an assessment of mortality risk should be made explicit to the patient and recorded clearly on the consent form and in the medical record” [21]. A spreadsheet has been developed to quantify mortal- ity risk before and after surgery by calculating mortality rates, and life expectancy with adjustment for var- ious parameters such as age, sex, co-morbidities, renal function, physical fitness, and body mass index [22]. An on-line calculator is available for estimating peri-opera- tive mortality in order to assist shared decision-making between patients and their doctors when non-surgical intervention is an option (see https://sites.google.com/ site/informrisk/). The complex nature of the peri-operative period gives rise to the potential for significant risk to all patients. Directing efforts towards patient safety can be uncomplicated and inexpensive, yet significantly improve the quality of peri-operative care. The World Health Organization’s surgical safety checklist is an example of a simple, cheap and effective method of reducing avoidable complications resulting from surgery [23]. More- over, the use of a checklist is likely to provide a net financial benefit to the healthcare system because the cost of the intervention is low under all scenarios and there should be a reduction in morbidity and medicolegal claims. Anaesthesia is a medical discipline of applied science related to the art of peri-operative risk reduction by identification, intervention, and prevention. To test or not to test High-risk patients account for no more than 15% of all surgical pro- cedures but over 80% of deaths [24]. However, it is still a major challenge to identify accurately and reliably patients who are at high risk of postoperative mortality and morbidity. Cardiopulmonary exer- cise testing (CPET) is a measure of aerobic capacity that is becoming more widely employed, with the estimated number of tests per- formed in the UK alone estimated to be in excess of 14 000 per year [25]. Based on the possible associa- tion of pre-operative aerobic fitness with subsequent survival after surgery, CPET has been used for triaging patients with occult cardio- respiratory disease for further investigation and optimisation strat- egies before major operations [26]. Nonetheless, none of the derived variables, such as ventilatory anaer- obic threshold or maximal oxygen uptake, can be regarded as a cor- nerstone for the prediction of sur- vival after major surgery [27]. The European Society of Cardiology’s guidelines on pre-operative cardiac risk assessment and peri-operative cardiac management, published in 2009, questioned the role of CPET in risk assessment before surgery and emphasised that it is not a sub- stitute for stress testing in routine practice [28]. A scientific statement from the American Heart Associa- tion has also highlighted the lack of randomised trials to support recom- mendations for diagnostic and prognostic applications of CPET [29]. Reliance upon any single fac- tor in predicting future risk is more like gambling than rational assess- ment, since no test is infallible. No single variable can estimate the extent of survival and quality of life, although physical fitness appears to be an important component [30, 31]. Pre-operative testing should not be a screening exercise for sta- ble patients but a strategic part of the peri-operative risk reduction programme for susceptible patients. Risk assessment should always be tailored to individual patients, and pre-operative tests only reserved for those in whom test results would positively influence and change peri-operative management. Our drive to ‘optimise risk’ can lead to unnecessary investigation and inter- vention when we should really be optimising ‘risk assessment’ through more comprehensive history taking and physical examination. Concerns over malpractice liability result in excessive and unnecessary consulta- © 2014 The Association of Anaesthetists of Great Britain and Ireland 1301 Editorial Anaesthesia 2014, 69, 1299–1313 tion, hospitalisation, testing, and treatment can, paradoxically, be a safety hazard for both patients and doctors, with false positive findings leading to costly and possibly harm- ful treatments or further investiga- tions and delays in surgery. The quantity of tests should not be con- fused with the quality of care. Are drugs the answer? Is there a pharmacological panacea for peri-operative risk optimisation? One of the main objectives of risk identification is to determine which individuals could benefit from a protective, therapeutic intervention. If that intervention is potentially dangerous in itself, e.g. coronary artery stenting, then careful selec- tion is absolutely essential. There are, however, fairly safe and simple pharmacological treatments that are very promising in this regard. It may not be necessary to investigate patients aggressively with expensive and even hazardous techniques if the indication for surgery is very strong. Why not assume the worst and instigate protective measures anyway? Over the last decade, sta- tins have been investigated exten- sively for their potential multimodal effects in modifying a number of aspects of peri-operative morbidity and mortality [32–34]. Possible pleiotropic effects of statin therapy are reduction of myocardial infarc- tion and stroke, prevention of atrial fibrillation, improvement of vascu- lar draft survival, protection from renal insufficiency, and inhibition of malignant cell growth [34]. All peri-operative applications of statins are ‘off-label’, as their primary indi- cation is lipid-lowering, but the drugs, which are now available in generic form, are relatively inexpen- sive with a very good safety profile [35]. However, in the latest Cochra- ne review of statins for vascular surgery, there was insufficient evi- dence to conclude that use of sta- tins resulted in either a reduction or an increase in any of the out- comes examined. It was also observed that the widespread use of statins in the population now will make it difficult for researchers to undertake the large randomised tri- als needed to demonstrate any effect [36]. Peri-operative pharmacological interventions, while having certain benefits, may themselves be associ- ated with risk that could outweigh such advantages. Data from the POISE trial suggested that routine administration of peri-operative beta-blockers in an un-titrated, rela- tively high dose starting on the day of operation, increased the risk of stroke and overall mortality for non-cardiac surgery in the presence of favourable outcomes on other cardiovascular parameters [37, 38]. POISE II has generated similar con- troversy recently over the use of peri-operative aspirin [39]. Conclusions Anaesthesia, as a service-based spe- ciality dealing with specific inci- dent-related risks, has not yet reached its peak despite being acknowledged as the leading medi- cal speciality in designing fail-secure systems and probably the only spe- cialty in healthcare to have reached the critical target of six sigma defect rate [6, 40]. The peri-operative period could be made safer by revolutionising risk management tactics with corresponding interven- tions in preventing complications and improving patient outcomes. It has been suggested that the techni- cal aspects of anaesthesia could be delegated to robots [41], so are we ready for our new role as peri- operative risk strategists? Competing interests No external funding and no com- peting interests declared. M. G. Irwin Professor V. K. F. Kong Honorary Assistant Professor Department of Anaesthesiology University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong Email: mgirwin@hku.hk References 1. Hauck K, Zhao X. How dangerous is a day in hospital? a model of adverse events and length of stay for medical inpatients. Medical Care 2011; 49: 1068–75. 2. Li G, Baker SP. CRash risk in general aviation. Journal of the American Medi- cal Association 2007; 297: 1596–8. 3. Li G, Warner M, Lang BH, Huang L, Sun LS. Epidemiology of anesthesia-related mortality in the United States, 1999– 2005. Anesthesiology 2009; 110: 759– 65. 4. Jenkins K, Baker AB. Consent and anaesthetic risk. Anaesthesia 2003; 58: 962–84. 5. Pearse RM, Moreno RP, Bauer P, et al. Mortality after surgery in Europe: a 7 day cohort study. Lancet 2012; 380: 1059–65. 6. Leape LL. Error in medicine. Journal of the American Medical Association 1994; 272: 1851–7. 7. Howard RA. Microrisks for medical decision analysis. International Journal of Technology Assessment in Health Care 1989; 5: 357–70. 8. Aitkenhead AR. Injuries associated with anaesthesia. A global perspective. Brit- ish Journal of Anaesthesia 2005; 95: 95–109. 1302 © 2014 The Association of Anaesthetists of Great Britain and Ireland Anaesthesia 2014, 69, 1299–1313 Editorial 9. Spiegelhalter D. Using speed of ageing and microlives to communicate the effects of lifetime habits and environ- ment. British Medical Journal 2012; 345: e8223. 10. Galesic M, Garcia-Retamero R. Using analogies to communicate information about health risks. Applied Cognitive Psychology 2013; 27: 33–42. 11. Lipkus IM, Samsa G, Rimer BK. General performance on a numeracy scale among highly educated samples. Medi- cal Decision Making 2001; 21: 37–44. 12. Peters E. Beyond comprehension: the role of numeracy in judgments and decisions. Current Directions in Psycho- logical Science 2012; 21: 31–5. 13. Cokely ET, Galesic M, Schulz E, Ghazal S, Garcia-Retamero R. Measuring risk literacy: the Berlin numeracy test. Judgment and Decision Making 2012; 7: 25–47. 14. Smith A. Foreword to the papers on ’The communication of risk’. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series A (Statistics in Society) 2003; 166: 205–6. 15. Edwards A. Communicating risks. Brit- ish Journal of Anaesthesia 2003; 327: 691–2. 16. Gigerenzer G, Gaissmaier W, Kurz-Mil- cke E, Schwartz LM, Woloshin S. Help- ing doctors and patients make sense of health statistics. Psychological Sci- ence in the Public Interest 2007; 8: 53–96. 17. Visschers VHM, Meertens RM, Passchier WWF, De Vries NNK. Probability infor- mation in risk communication: a review of the research literature. Risk Analysis 2009; 29: 267–87. 18. Furedi A. The public health implications of the 1995 ‘pill scare’. Human Repro- duction Update 1999; 5: 621–6. 19. Schwartz LM, Woloshin S, Dvorin EL, Welch HG. Ratio measures in leading medical journals: structured review of accessibility of underlying absolute risks. British Medical Journal 2006; 333: 1248. 20. Sedrakyan A, Shih C. Improving depic- tion of benefits and harms: analyses of studies of well-known therapeutics and review of high-impact medical journals. Medical Care 2007; 45: S23–S8. 21. Findley G, Goodwin A, Protopapa K, Smith N, Mason M. Knowing the risk: a review of the peri-operative care of sur- gical patients. London: NCEPOD, 2011. 22. Carlisle JB. Pre-operative co-morbidity and postoperative survival in the elderly: beyond one lunar orbit. Anaes- thesia 2014; 69: 17–25. 23. Haynes AB, Weiser TG, Berry WR, et al. A surgical safety checklist to reduce morbidity and mortality in a global population. New England Journal of Medicine 2009; 360: 491–9. 24. Pearse R, Harrison D, James P, et al. Identification and characterisation of the high-risk surgical population in the United Kingdom. Critical Care 2006; 10: R81. 25. Huddart S, Young EL, Smith R-L, Holt PJ, Prabhu PK. Preoperative cardiopul- monary exercise testing in England–a national survey. Perioperative Medicine 2013; 2: 4. 26. Kothmann E, Danjoux G, Owen SJ, Parry A, Turley AJ, Batterham AM. Reli- ability of the anaerobic threshold in cardiopulmonary exercise testing of patients with abdominal aortic aneu- rysms. Anaesthesia 2009; 64: 9–13. 27. Colson M, Baglin J, Bolsin S, Grocott MPW. Cardiopulmonary exercise testing predicts 5 yr survival after major sur- gery. British Journal of Anaesthesia 2012; 109: 735–41. 28. Poldermans D, Bax JJ, Boersma E, et al. Guidelines for pre-operative cardiac risk assessment and perioperative car- diac management in non-cardiac sur- gery: The Task Force for Preoperative Cardiac Risk Assessment and Perioper- ative Cardiac Management in Non-car- diac Surgery of the European Society of Cardiology (ESC) and endorsed by the European Society of Anaesthesiology (ESA). European Heart Journal 2009; 30: 2769–812. 29. Balady GJ, Arena R, Sietsema K, et al. Clini- cian’s guide to cardiopulmonary exercise testing in adults: a scientific statement from the American Heart Association. Cir- culation 2010; 122: 191–225. 30. Carlisle JB. The quick and the dead. Anaesthesia 2013; 68: 799–803. 31. Lee L, Schwartzman K, Carli F, et al. The association of the distance walked in 6 min with pre-operative peak oxy- gen consumption and complications 1 month after colorectal resection. Anaesthesia 2013; 68: 811–6. 32. Biccard BM. A peri-operative statin update for non-cardiac surgery. Part II: Statin therapy for vascular surgery and peri-operative statin trial design. Anaesthesia 2008; 63: 162–71. 33. Biccard BM. A peri-operative statin update for non-cardiac surgery. Part I: The effects of statin therapy on athero- sclerotic disease and lessons learnt from statin therapy in medical (non- surgical) patients. Anaesthesia 2008; 63: 52–64. 34. Chan WW, Wong GT, Irwin MG. Periop- erative statin therapy. Expert Opinion on Pharmacotherapy 2013; 14: 831– 42. 35. Minder CM, Blumenthal RS, Blaha MJ. Statins for primary prevention of car- diovascular disease: the benefits out- weigh the risks. Current Opinion in Cardiology 2013; 28: 554–60. 36. Sanders RD, Nicholson A, Lewis SR, Smith AF, Alderson P. Perioperative sta- tin therapy for improving outcomes during and after noncardiac vascular surgery. Cochrane Database of System- atic Reviews 2013; 7: CD009971. 37. Devereaux P, Yang H, Yusuf S, et al. Effects of extended-release metoprolol succinate in patients undergoing non- cardiac surgery (POISE trial): a rando- mised controlled trial. Lancet 2008; 371: 1839–47. 38. Wijeysundera DN, Duncan D, Nkonde- Price C, et al. Perioperative beta block- ade in noncardiac surgery: a systematic review for the 2014 ACC/AHA Guide- line on Perioperative Cardiovascular Evaluation and Management of Patients Undergoing Noncardiac Sur- gery: a report of the American College of Cardiology/American Heart Associa- tion Task Force on Practice Guidelines. Journal of the American College of Car- diology 2014 Aug 1; doi 10.1016/j. jacc.2014.07.939. 39. Devereaux PJ, Mrkobrada M, Sessler DI, et al. Aspirin in patients undergoing noncardiac surgery. New England Jour- nal of Medicine 2014; 370: 1494–503. 40. Amalberti R, Auroy Y, Berwick D, Bar- ach P. Five system barriers to achieving ultrasafe health care. Annals of Inter- nal Medicine 2005; 142: 756–64. 41. Hemmerling TM, Terrasini N. Robotic anesthesia: not the realm of science fiction any more. Current Opinion in Anesthesiology 2012; 25: 736–42. doi:10.1111/anae.12881 © 2014 The Association of Anaesthetists of Great Britain and Ireland 1303 Editorial Anaesthesia 2014, 69, 1299–1313