key: cord-279214-7vna4uyo authors: Goldfarb, Elizabeth V. title: Participant stress in the COVID-19 era and beyond date: 2020-09-25 journal: Nat Rev Neurosci DOI: 10.1038/s41583-020-00388-7 sha: doc_id: 279214 cord_uid: 7vna4uyo The COVID-19 pandemic represents a worldwide stressor. Considering the influence of stress on research participants during this time and beyond may provide new insights and benefit the broader field of human neuroscience. As human neuroscience researchers, we invite people, with their disparate backgrounds, to participate in experi ments to help us uncover fundamental truths about the brain. We are now in the midst of a global pandemic that is changing our lives and the lives of our participants. For those of us who are currently able to work with human participants (in person or online), stress related to the pandemic may have farreaching and unanticipated effects on the data collected during this period. Although the pandemic highlights the importance of stress as a factor in everyday life, stress and its neuro biological sequelae are not unique to the pandemic. Indeed, stress can be routinely considered and measured in human neuroscience research. However, outside studies explicitly designed to measure stress or affec tive processes, stress is not typically considered. Here, I discuss why and how stress should be considered in the design and interpretation of human neuroscience studies during the pandemic and moving forward. The stress of a pandemic Stress is classically described as a response to something novel, unpredictable and uncontrollable -all features of the COVID19 pandemic. The fear of illness for ourselves and our loved ones for an unknown period of time has been combined with, for example, prolonged disruptions to daily routine, education, child care, occupation and income that have resulted from efforts to limit the spread of the virus 1 . In the United States, these challenges have been disproportionately faced by Black and Indigenous people and other people of colour, who are also systema tically exposed to more stressful experiences 1,2 . Isolation and lack of social support can also trigger maladap tive coping behaviours such as increased substance use that can potentiate risk for COVID19 related complications 3 and interact with stress pathways to alter brain circuitry and cognitive function 4 . If having participants put their arm in a bucket of icy water for a few minutes (a common laboratory stress induction protocol) is sufficient to change participants' behaviour and brain responses 5 , one can imagine the scale of the impact that COVID19 related stress may have on the data being collected now. Stress leads to changes in neuronal structure and function throughout the brain. These effects are espe cially pronounced when a stressor is experienced repeat edly or over a long time period, known as chronic stress (which may characterize participants' experiences of the pandemic). Research across different species has shown that chronic stress impairs prefrontal and hippocampal circuits (inducing dendritic atrophy and spine loss), and strengthens responses in the amygdala 6, 7 . These changes occur together with effects on myriad cognitive pro cesses, including attention, reward processing, learning, working memory, long term memory, decision making, strategy selection and top down regulation of emotion 8 . The clinical research community has emphasized the importance of considering the pandemic as a multi dimensional stressor to understand mental health consequences 1 . Here, I suggest that this consideration extends beyond clinical science: all the data that are cur rently being collected from human participants are likely to be influenced by this sustained stress exposure. When designing studies to examine stress effects, a 'control group' is typically included to match a 'stress group' in as many ways as possible, short of the stress exposure. This enables us to draw conclusions about the specific con tributions of stress to the behavioural construct or neural process of interest. However, as practically all humans are now exposed to the pandemic, there is no temporally matched control group (although longitudinal studies may be able to consider pre pandemic versus post pandemic differences). Crucially, experiences of and reactions to the challenges associated with the pandemic are highly vari able. For example, one participant may have been socially isolated yet had no changes in their employment; another may have grieved the loss of a loved one, taken on new caregiving roles, experienced income loss and increased their alcohol use; and yet another may have had fewer commitments and enjoyed more time with family. To better understand human data, and how they may be modulated by stressors like the pandemic, researchers can leverage tools developed by the stress field 9 . Subjective measures. Several well validated instruments have been developed to measure the number of stressors people experience and the extent of psychological dis tress they cause, typically including an assessment of when they occurred during the lifespan 10 . These can be implemented using self report or interviews by trained personnel. More recently, techniques such as ecolo gical momentary assessment (EMA) use smartphone prompts to track daily fluctuations in experiences of and responses to stressful events. Coping behaviours are also relevant to measure, as some responses, such as escala ting alcohol or drug use, can in turn influence behaviour and brain responses 4 . Although self report measures rely on conscious insight and willingness to describe what may be stigmatized responses, they can help to predict later health outcomes 9 and can feasibly be incorporated into online and in person experiments. Testing kits are widely used to measure stress related hormones in samples of saliva, plasma or hair. Although salivary and plasma cortisol are frequently used to measure acute stress responses in the laboratory, they can also provide insight into more prolonged stress states. For example, basal cortisol levels at different times of day are altered with past stress exposure 10 , stress related psychopathology 8 and chronic drug use 4 . Although obtaining saliva and plasma (poten tially infectious agents) may be challenging during the pandemic, these measures are routinely used in the stress field, providing a rich comparison data set against which to interpret new data. The body's stress response can also be assessed through measures of autonomic function. Tools to measure autonomic responses include arm cuffs to quantify systolic and diastolic blood pressure, as well as electrocardiograms (ECGs) that can provide ambulatory indices of pulse and heart rate variability. These auto nomic functions also adapt with chronic stress and drug use 4, 9 . Given the lack of need for bodily fluid samples to assess autonomic responses, such measures would prob ably be more practicable than neuroendocrine analyses during the pandemic. Acute stress challenges. Laboratory stressors are frequently used as experimental interventions and can also be used as assays for adaptations in acute stress responses result ing from chronic stress exposure. Participants' reactivity profiles to acute stress can be heightened, blunted or show atypical recovery patterns as a function of their stress histories and biological backgrounds 9 . There are various techniques for inducing stress responses in the labora tory, including physical, psychological and cogni tive chal lenges 5 . By measuring subjective, neuroendocrine and/or autonomic responses before and after such procedures, researchers can quantify acute stress reactivity. Beyond studies explicitly designed to examine effects of stress, obtaining stress measures provides opportunities for novel insights into brain and behavioural data. Careful assessment of individuals' stress profiles may enable researchers to account for unanticipated sources of variance. This approach can also allow researchers to interpret indivi dual differences and divide participants into meaning ful cohorts, leading to the discovery of novel ways in which neural and behavioural processes are associated with stress. The need to consider the affective and physiological context in which people participate in research does not end with the end of COVID19. We cannot assume that participants always complete our experiments in a neutral state, or that they have comparable histo ries of stressful experiences. Individual differences in stress and coping might explain hitherto unknown boundary conditions for reproducing effects in human neuro science, and improve ecological validity by indicating how these processes may occur in the stress rich context of everyday life. Going forward, participant level stress profiles can include assessment of remote stress experiences (including the pandemic) as well as in the experimental moment stress reacti vity. Broadly consi dering potential stress effects provides opportunities to identify more brain functions that may be susce ptible to stress, elucidate neural factors that determine whether stress is associated with enhanced or impaired function 5 and uncover protective processes that promote resilience. Knowing what we do about how stress alters fundamental neural processes, examining the effects of the pandemic and other stressors on data from human participants creates an opening to gain crucial insight into the mechanisms of the human brain. Mental health and clinical psychological science in the time of COVID-19: Challenges, opportunities, and a call to action Assessment of COVID-19 hospitalizations by race/ethnicity in 12 states Collision of the COVID-19 and addiction epidemics Drug-induced stress responses and addiction risk and relapse Enhancing memory with stress: progress, challenges, and opportunities Stress weakens prefrontal networks: molecular insults to higher cognition Stress effects on neuronal structure: hippocampus, amygdala, and prefrontal cortex The effects of chronic stress on the human brain: from neurotoxicity, to vulnerability, to opportunity More than a feeling: a unified view of stress measurement for population science Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition The author gratefully acknowledges M. D. Rosenberg and D. V. Clewett for helpful discussions. The authors declare no competing interests.