key: cord-307520-8zmwh9ch authors: Smith, L.E.; Amlȏt, R.; Lambert, H.; Oliver, I.; Robin, C.; Yardley, L.; Rubin, G.J. title: Factors associated with adherence to self-isolation and lockdown measures in the UK: a cross-sectional survey date: 2020-09-06 journal: Public Health DOI: 10.1016/j.puhe.2020.07.024 sha: doc_id: 307520 cord_uid: 8zmwh9ch OBJECTIVES: To investigate factors associated with adherence to self-isolation and lockdown measures due to COVID-19 in the UK. STUDY DESIGN: Online cross-sectional survey. METHODS: Data were collected between 6th and 7th May 2020. A total of 2240 participants living in the UK aged 18 years or older were recruited from YouGov's online research panel. RESULTS: A total of 217 people (9.7%) reported that they or someone in their household had symptoms of COVID-19 (cough or high temperature/fever) in the last 7 days. Of these people, 75.1% had left the home in the last 24 h (defined as non-adherent). Men were more likely to be non-adherent, as were people who were less worried about COVID-19, and who perceived a smaller risk of catching COVID-19. Adherence was associated with having received help from someone outside your household. Results should be taken with caution as there was no evidence for associations when controlling for multiple analyses. Of people reporting no symptoms in the household, 24.5% had gone out shopping for non-essentials in the last week (defined as non-adherent). Factors associated with non-adherence and with a higher total number of outings in the last week included decreased perceived effectiveness of government ‘lockdown’ measures, decreased perceived severity of COVID-19 and decreased estimates of how many other people were following lockdown rules. Having received help was associated with better adherence. CONCLUSIONS: Adherence to self-isolation is poor. As we move into a new phase of contact tracing and self-isolation, it is essential that adherence is improved. Communications should aim to increase knowledge about actions to take when symptomatic or if you have been in contact with a possible COVID-19 case. They should also emphasise the risk of catching and spreading COVID-19 when out and about and the effectiveness of preventative measures. Using volunteer networks effectively to support people in isolation may promote adherence. During the coronavirus pandemic, governments have imposed restrictions of movement to prevent the spread of the virus. Commonly used measures are self-isolation, in which people who are ill separate themselves from others, and quarantine, in which people who may have been exposed to the illness separate themselves from others. 1 On 23rd March 2020, the UK government introduced 'lockdown' measures to slow the spread of COVID-19. 2, 3 These required people to: stay at home except for several, limited reasons; not leave the home at all for 7 days, if suffering from a new continuous cough or fever; and not leave the home at all for 14 days, if someone else in the household developed cough or fever. Adherence to these measures may be influenced by multiple factors. According to Protection Motivation Theory, 4 uptake of a protective behaviour is influenced by your appraisal of a threat, including its severity and your susceptibility to it, and your appraisal of the behaviour, including perceptions about its efficacy, your ability to perform it and the costs associated with it. A review of quarantine measures in previous public health crises found that knowledge and perceived social norms were also associated with adherence to quarantine. 5 Conversely, fear of missing out, perceived social pressure, perceived legal consequences, running out of supplies (e.g. food or medicine) and financial pressures were associated with decreased adherence. There is some evidence that people who think they have had COVID-19 are less likely to adhere to lockdown measures. 6 In this study, we investigated factors associated with adherence to lockdown measures in a demographically representative sample of the UK adult population. We commissioned the market research company YouGov to carry out this cross-sectional survey, between 6th and 7th May 2020. Participants (n ¼ 2240) were recruited from YouGov's online research panel (n ¼ 800,000þ UK adults) and were eligible if they aged 18 years or older and living in the UK. Quota sampling was used, based on age, gender, social grade, level of education and Government Office Region, to ensure that the sample was broadly representative of the UK general population. In total, 74 participants were excluded because of a lack of data for sociodemographic variables, suspiciously fast completion of the survey or providing identical answers to multiple consecutive questions. Participants were reimbursed in points equivalent to approximately 50p. Full survey materials are available in the Supplementary Materials. We asked participants how many times they had left their home 'in the past 24 h' and 'in the past seven days': to go to the shops for groceries, toiletries or medicine; to go to the shops for other items; for exercise; for a medical purpose excluding going to the shops/ pharmacy for medicine; to go to work; to help someone else; and to meet friends or family who they did not live with. We asked participants if they or a household member had experienced any of 13 symptoms, including cough and high temperature/fever, in the past 7 and 14 days, respectively. We asked participants whether they thought they had 'had, or currently have, coronavirus' and if they were currently selfisolating. We asked participants a series of true/false statements about the current UK government guidance. We asked participants how worried they were about COVID-19 on a five-point Likert-type scale from 'not at all worried' to 'extremely worried'. To measure perceived social norms, we asked participants to estimate the percentage of people the same age as them who were fully following the UK government's recommendations to stay at home. We asked participants whether they thought the current lockdown had made their physical health better or worse. Possible answers were 'a lot better', 'a little better', 'no difference', 'a little worse' and 'a lot worse'. We asked participants to rate their general health on a fivepoint Likert-type scale from 'poor' to 'excellent' using one item from the SF-36. 7 Table 1 Participants' personal and clinical characteristics, by report of symptoms in the household. We asked participants if they had helped someone, or received help from someone, outside their household in the past 7 days (yes/ no). We asked participants to rate 14 perception statements on a five-point Likert scale from 'strongly disagree' to 'strongly agree'. Statements included the perceived severity of COVID-19, perceived effectiveness of government measures, perceived likelihood of catching and spreading COVID-19, perceived costs of following government measures, fear of losing touch with friends and relatives, social pressure from friends and family to follow government measures, perceived legal consequences of not following government measures and positive consequences of the lockdown. We asked participants to report their age, gender, employment status, highest educational or professional qualification and marital status. We also asked whether there was a child in their household, whether they or someone else in their household received a letter from the National Health Service telling them they were extremely clinically vulnerable to COVID-19, and whether they lived alone. Participants were asked for their postcode to determine indices of multiple deprivation (IMD) and whether they lived in an urban or rural area. We also collected social grade. We asked participants if their primary home had access to any outdoor space, and whether they were pet owners. Ethical approval for this study was granted by the King's College London Research Ethics Committee (reference: LRS-19/20-18687). We calculated achieved power for the analyses (in households with and without symptoms) using post-hoc power calculations. Achieved power is presented underneath relevant analyses. For all variables, unless stated otherwise, we coded answers of 'don't know' as missing data. We investigated whether out-of-home activity (total number of outings, percentage of people reporting shopping for nonessentials, going to meet friends or family, and having visitors to their home) differed by presence of symptoms in the household. We split the sample by presence of symptoms in the household. Among those who reported symptoms in their household in the last 7 days, we defined those who reported having gone out in the last 24 h as not adhering to self-isolation measures. We ran a series of logistic regressions investigating univariable associations between personal and clinical factors, psychological and situational factors, and having left the home in the past 24 h. We ran a second set of logistic regressions controlling for personal and clinical characteristics. Among those who reported no symptoms in the household, we used UK government guidelines that were in force at the time of data collection 2 to define non-adherence (shopping for nonessentials, meeting friends or family and having visitors to your home). We ran a series of linear regressions investigating univariable associations between personal and clinical factors, psychological and situational factors, and total number of outings reported in the past 7 days. We ran a second set of linear regressions controlling for personal and clinical characteristics (personal and clinical characteristics entered as the first block, other independent variables as the second block). We ran a series of logistic regressions investigating univariable associations between personal and clinical factors, psychological and situational factors, and going out shopping for items other than groceries, toiletries or medicines (non-essentials) in the past 7 days. We ran a second set of logistic regressions controlling for personal and clinical characteristics. Weighting data by age, gender, social grade, highest level of education and region altered prevalence of outcome behaviours only slightly. We therefore used unweighted data in our analyses. Owing to the large number of analyses (n ¼ 39) run on each outcome, we applied a Bonferroni correction to our results (P 0.001). Those meeting this criterion are marked by a double asterisk (**) in the tables. Results of adjusted analyses are reported narratively; unadjusted results are reported in tables. A minority of participants (9.7%, n ¼ 217) reported that either they or a household member had a cough or a high temperature/ fever in the last 7 or 14 days, respectively. Participants' characteristics are shown in Table 1 . Male participants were more likely to report symptoms in their household. There were no other differences between groups. Of participants who reported symptoms in their household (n ¼ 217), 75.1% (n ¼ 163, 95% confidence interval [CI; 69.3e80.9]) reported leaving the home at least once in the past 24 h. This finding has been reported elsewhere. 8 A few participants (n ¼ 54, 2.4%) reported going out many times; we grouped responses of over 20 times in the past 7 days. There was no difference in out-of-home activity by presence of symptoms in the household (total number of outings made in the last week, t(2160) ¼ 0.20, P ¼ .84; percentage of people reporting shopping for non-essentials, c 2 (1, 2162) ¼ 0.38, P ¼ .54; having had a visitor to one's home, c 2 (1, 2076) ¼ 0.40, P ¼ .53; or going to meet friends or family, c 2 (1, 2162) ¼ 1.34, P ¼ .25). Of those who reported symptoms in the household, 34.1% (n ¼ 74) reported that they were self-isolating. Of those 'selfisolating', 60.8% (n ¼ 45) nonetheless reported having gone out in the last 24 h. Men were more likely to leave the home in the last 24 h (see Table 2 ). Non-adherence to self-isolation (reporting having left home in the last 24 h) was associated with: thinking that the lockdown had made your mental health worse; feeling a greater sense of community with your neighbourhood due to COVID-19 (see Table 3 ). 13.89 0.007 <0.001** À0.08 <0.001** À0.09 <0.001** Age, years 18e24, n ¼ 76 For analyses where symptoms were present in the household, we achieved 94% power to detect small effect sizes in logistic regression analyses (odds ratio [OR] ¼ 1.68, 9 a ¼ .05, sample size n ¼ 217, probability of having left the home ¼ 0.75, one-tailed logistic regression; 89% power when using a two-tailed logistic regression). 10 Of those who reported no symptoms in their household, 24 Personal and clinical factors (gender, age, having a child in the household, being extremely clinically vulnerable oneself, employment status, highest level of education or professional qualification, IMD, social grade, living in a rural or urban area, living alone, marital status and region [results for region not reported]) explained 8.0% of the variance in number of outings in the past week (see Table 4 ). More outings were made by men, those who reported working and who lived in rural areas. Fewer outings were made by those who were clinically extremely vulnerable and who lived in more deprived areas. Having a pet was also associated with going out more often. More outings in the past week were associated with: helping someone outside your household; decreased perceived effectiveness of government measures; thinking that you would lose touch with friends and relatives if you followed government advice; not enjoying spending more time at home during the lockdown; better self-reported general health; decreased perceived severity of COVID-19; decreased perceived likelihood of spreading COVID-19; decreased perceived legal consequences of not following government advice; decreased perceived social pressure from friends and family to follow government measures; full, correct knowledge of government measures if no-one in the household was symptomatic; believing that you have had or currently have COVID-19; increased perceived financial cost of following government measures; and decreased perceived social norms (see Table 5 ). Fewer outings were associated with: receiving help from someone outside your household; decreased perceived impact of lockdown on physical health; reporting that you were self-isolating; increased worry about COVID-19; and increased perceived likelihood of catching COVID-19. Going out shopping for non-essentials in the past week was associated with male participants, working and lower social grade (see Table 6 ). Shopping for non-essentials in the past week was associated with: thinking you have had COVID-19; helping someone outside your household; thinking that you will lose touch with friends or relatives if you follow government guidance; and thinking that following government guidance will negatively impact you financially (see Table 7 ). Not going out shopping for non-essentials was associated with: having received help from someone outside your household in the last 7 days; reporting that you were self-isolating; increased perceived likelihood of catching and spreading COVID-19; increased worry about COVID-19; increased perceived effectiveness of government advice; increased perceived severity of COVID-19; increased perceived disapproval from friends or family if you do not follow government advice; increased perceived legal consequences of not following government advice; not knowing or being unsure about government measures; and decreased perceived social norms. For analyses where no symptoms were present in the household, we achieved 100% power to detect small effect sizes in logistic regression analyses (OR ¼ 1.68, 9 a ¼ .05, sample size n ¼ 1945, probability of having gone out shopping for items other than groceries, toiletries or medicines ¼ 0.25, one-tailed and two-tailed logistic regression). We achieved 94% power to detect small effect sizes in linear regression analyses (f 2 ¼ 0.02, 10 a ¼ .05, sample size n ¼ 1945, number of tested predictors ¼ 39, total number of predictors ¼ 39). To the best of our knowledge, this is the first comprehensive study to investigate factors associated with self-isolation and behaviour during lockdown in the UK. Almost 10% of participants reported that either they or a household member had symptoms of COVID-19 (a cough or high temperature/fever) in the last week. Prevalence estimates by the UK Office for National Statistics indicate that at the time of data collection, 0.27% of the community population had COVID-19. 11 Government regulations required all those with symptoms, or with symptoms in their household, to self-isolate. Our results suggest that adherence to this is poor. Three-quarters of those with symptoms in their household reported leaving their home in the past 24 h. We found no difference in out-of-home behaviour by presence of symptoms in the household. The UK will shortly enter a new phase of the pandemic, in which extensive testing, contact tracing and isolation will be required to keep the spread of COVID-19 in check. 12 For this to succeed, adherence must be improved. There is some evidence that institution-based isolation is more effective compared to homebased isolation, in part because this is less reliant on personal adherence to guidelines. 13 Some countries have used large-scale, temporary shelter hospitals, which are primarily for patients with mild and moderate symptoms of COVID-19. Shelter hospitals allow patients to isolate effectively from their family and community; be triaged, reducing pressure on other health care services; provide basic medical care; frequent monitoring and rapid referral if a patients' symptoms worsen; and provide living and social support. 14 Our findings highlight several risk factors for poor adherence. Notably men were more likely to report having been out in the last 24 h if they or someone in their household was symptomatic, having gone out more times in the last week and shopping for nonessentials. Lower adherence among men was also noted in the UK during the 2009/10 H1N1 influenza pandemic. 15 Communication campaigns that specifically target men may therefore have merit. Adherence with self-isolation was associated with increased worry about COVID-19 and increased perceived likelihood of catching COVID-19. As incidence declines, it is possible that worry will also decline, reducing adherence further. Although it may be tempting to use fear-based messaging to combat this, this may influence other behaviours that the government may wish to encourage, such as return to work. 16 Adherence was also associated with having received help from someone outside your household. This makes intuitive Table 5 Associations between psychological and situational factors and total number of outings in the past week in participants who reported no symptoms in the household. Because of the current lockdown, there is more conflict between people that I live with 5-point scale, 1 ¼ strongly disagree to 5 ¼ strongly agree, n ¼ 1859 If I follow the government's advice, I will not be able to carry out important religious activities 5-point scale, 1 ¼ strongly disagree to 5 ¼ strongly agree, n ¼ 1719 Associations between personal and clinical characteristics of participants who reported no symptoms in their household in the last week and having gone shopping for items other than groceries, toiletries or medicines (non-essentials). Had not gone out shopping for nonessentials; n ¼ 1469, n (%) Had gone out shopping for nonessentials; n ¼ 476, n (%) sensedhaving someone else to run errands should reduce the need for you to leave home. Much has been made recently of the remarkable altruism of 750,000 people who signed-up to volunteer for the National Health Service, and the lack of jobs for them to do. 17 Allowing those in self-isolation to submit requests for help may be a pragmatic way to improve adherence. Adherence to lockdown measures among those not reporting symptoms in their household was better, but still not perfect, with 75% reporting not going out to shop for non-essential items. Percentages reporting not meeting up with friends or family from outside one's household and not having visitors to the home were higher (94% and 95%, respectively). Adherence was lower in men and those who reported working. It is plausible that workers may be more likely to be out and about for work and while out, go shopping for non-essentials. Those working may also be more financially able to shop for non-essential items. Although perceiving greater negative financial consequences of government measures was associated with non-adherence to lockdown measures, there was no longer evidence for an association after correcting for multiple adjustments. This is different from research finding decreased intention to adhere to quarantine measures in Israel. 18 Adherence to lockdown measures was also associated with higher threat appraisals and positive appraisals of the coping response. These findings mirror research in other countries. 19e21 Non-adherence was associated with decreased perceived social norms, 19, 22 lower perceived social pressure to adhere to measures and decreased knowledge of measures. 5 These findings suggest that improvement in adherence to lockdown measures is likely to be achieved by emphasising these are actions that most people are taking, that are having a positive impact, and that others around you want you to do. This study has several limitations. First, despite using quota sampling, we cannot be sure that survey respondents are representative of the general population. 23, 24 Second, all data were selfreported and may have been susceptible to social desirability bias. 25 However, preliminary data indicate that self-reported physical distancing is associated with real-world behaviour. 26 Third, we did not ask participants if they came into close contact with anyone from another household while they were out and about. Clearly, non-adherence does not always increase the risk of disease transmission. Fourth, we used a cumulative measure of 'outings' for our outcome measure. It is possible that participants may have shopped for essentials and non-essentials in the same trip, which might be double-counted in our questionnaire. Fifth, the cross-sectional nature of data collection means we are unable to draw causal inferences. Sixth, although the total sample size was large, a small percentage of the population reported that they or someone in their household had experienced symptoms of COVID-19 in the last week. Thus, analyses investigating adherence to selfisolation were based on smaller sample sizes, resulting in decreased power and wider confidence intervals. Overall, our data suggest that self-reported adherence to selfisolation measures was poor. This has important implications for policies that attempt to prevent the spread of COVID-19 through self-isolation, such as contact tracing. Psychological factors including perceived effectiveness of lockdown measures, should be emphasised in communications. Effective use of volunteer programmes and help within the neighbourhood or community may also improve adherence. Anonymised data will be made available on reasonable request. The study was conceptualised by RA, HL, IO, CR, LY and GJR. LS completed all analyses, using data from YouGov Plc. All authors contributed to, and approved, the final manuscript. For any enquiries about the data in this report please contact King's College London. Isolation, quarantine, social distancing and community containment: pivotal role for old-style public health measures in the novel coronavirus (2019-nCoV) outbreak New rules on staying at home and away from others PM address to the nation on coronavirus Protection motivation theory How to improve adherence with quarantine: rapid review of the evidence The impact of believing you have had COVID-19 on behaviour: cross-sectional survey The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-36) Conceptual framework and item selection Improving adherence to 'test, trace and isolate How big is a big odds ratio? Interpreting the magnitudes of odds ratios in epidemiological studies Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences Coronavirus (COVID-19) infection survey pilot: England Feasibility of controlling COVID-19 outbreaks by isolation of cases and contacts Institutional, not home-based, isolation could contain the COVID-19 outbreak Fangcang shelter hospitals: a novel concept for responding to public health emergencies Public perceptions, anxiety, and behaviour change in relation to the swine flu outbreak: cross sectional telephone survey The impact of communications about swine flu (influenza A H1N1v) on public responses to the outbreak: results from 36 national telephone surveys in the UK NHS coronavirus crisis volunteers frustrated at lack of tasks. The Guardian Self-isolation compliance in the COVID-19 era influenced by compensation: findings from A recent survey in Israel Social, cognitive, and emotional predictors of adherence to physical distancing during the COVID-19 pandemic Predictors of non-adherence to public health instructions during the COVID-19 pandemic COVID-19 is rapidly changing: examining public perceptions and behaviors in response to this evolving pandemic Social norms motivate COVID-19 preventive behaviours Researching internet-based populations advantages and disadvantages of online survey research, online questionnaire authoring software packages, and web survey services How to survey citizens' compliance with COVID-19 public health measures? Evidence from three survey experiments Connecting self-reported social distancing to real-world behavior at the individual and U.S. State level Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.puhe.2020.07.024.