Gustave Caillebotte, French Impressionism, and mere exposure Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2003, 10 (2), 319-343 Gustave Caillebotte was Impressionism’s anomaly, in his life as well as his art. . . . He was 14 years younger than his initial stylistic model Degas and when his eventual close friends Monet and Renoir first began plotting an in- dependent exhibition . . . he was still a teenager. . . . He was also rich. Varnedoe (1987, p. 1) Inheriting his father’s wealth, acquired as a military supplier, Gustave Caillebotte was a rich painter at age 28. Too young and freshly started for the first Impressionist exhibition in 1874, he was invited by Renoir to join the second in 1876, and he participated in four others. More- over, he organized the 1876 exhibit and subsidized it and those in 1877, 1879, and 1882. Most important in this con- text, he also began to buy paintings in 1876, supporting his friends when little other money was coming in (see Bérhaut, 1994, Distel, 1990, Marrinan, 2002, Nord, 2000, and Varnedoe, 1987, for histories). Seven of his purchases are shown in the left-hand panels of Figures 1 and 2; Caillebotte painted Figure 1A himself. Caillebotte’s major phase of acquisitions continued during the height of Im- pressionism.1 Aside from a few drawings, he collected only the works of Paul Cézanne, Edgar Degas, Edouard Manet, Claude Monet, Camille Pissarro, Pierre-Auguste Renoir, and Alfred Sisley. These painters are widely regarded as the “major” artists of Impressionism, and their works form the core of the Impressionist canon.2 L’AFFAIRE CAILLEBOTTE Caillebotte died suddenly in 1894 at age 46. His will left his entire art collection to the state of France, on the condition that the works be hung together for the public. Such a bequest was unprecedented, and the off icial salon culture of Paris was still ill disposed toward Impression- ism. Jean-Léon Gérôme, an important salon painter who was influential in the late 19th century Paris art scene, is reported to have said, “I do not know these gentlemen and of the donation I know only the title—Are there not some paintings of Monsieur Monet in it? Of Monsieur Pissarro and others? For the state to accept such f ilth would be a blot on morality” (cited in Mead, 1974, un- paginated; see also Rewald, 1946, p. 422).3 But perhaps as important as the politics was the unavailability of space; there seemed to be no suitable place in Paris large enough to hang the 73 or more paintings. The haggling went on for years, taxing the patience of Renoir, Caillebotte’s executor, and Martial Caillebotte, the older brother. Eventually, the will was broken, and the collection was split, 38 images going to the state of France and the rest rejected. Two of Caillebotte’s own paintings (Images 1c and 2c; see the Appendix) were in- cluded in the bequest by his family, and one Sisley (Image 60c) was later deaccessioned and sent to a re- gional museum. Thus, we can consider Caillebotte’s Parisian legacy to be 39 works. These were f irst hung in 319 Copyright 2003 Psychonomic Society, Inc. This research began as a set of interests accrued in 1993, when I was in Paris, supported by a John Simon Guggenheim Memorial Fellow- ship, and later, in the summer of 1994, during the Caillebotte centen- nial. I thank Claudia Lazzaro for being the final, if unwitting, impetus for this study in the summer of 2000—again, in Paris—and for many wonderful hours of discussion about art and canons; Justine Zee Kwok for beginning aspects of this project as a feasibility study, for many hours of library work, and for help with initial assembly of the data- bases; Nicholas Epley for initial guidance through the mere exposure literature; John Bargh and Arthur Reber for encouragement and com- ments; Andrea Coby Riddle, the teachers, and students at the Elizabeth Ann Clune Montessori School of Ithaca for their help with Study 3; David Dunning and Dennis Regan for suggestions leading to Studies 4 and 5; and many colleagues for their friendly cajoling, forcing me to de- sign and carry out Study 6. A version of Study 1 was presented at the 42nd Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, November 2001, in Orlando, FL. Correspondence concerning this article should be sent to J. E. Cutting, Department of Psychology, Uris Hall, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853-7601 (e-mail: jec7@cornell.edu). Gustave Caillebotte, French Impressionism, and mere exposure JAMES E. CUTTING Cornell University, Ithaca, New York Gustave Caillebotte was a painter, a collector of some of his colleagues’ most renowned works, and a major force in the creation of the late 19th century French Impressionist canon. Six studies are presented as a naturalistic investigation of the effects of mere exposure to images in his collection and to those matched to them. The probabilities of cultural exposure to the 132 stimulus images were indexed by the frequencies of their separate appearances in Cornell University library books—a total of 4,232 times in 980 different books. Across the studies, adult preferences were correlated with differences in image frequencies, but not with recognition, complexity, or prototypicality judgments; children’s preferences were not correlated with frequency. Prior cultural exposure also interacted with experimental expo- sure in predictable ways. The results suggest that mere exposure helps to maintain an artistic canon. 320 CUTTING Figure 1. Eight Impressionist images. The four in the left column were part of the Caille- botte legacy to the state of France; the four on the right were m atched to them for these stud- ies. All four pairs were used as part of a corpus of 132 images. Panels A and B are Gustave Caillebotte’s Les Raboteurs de parquet (“Floor scrapers,” Im age 1a; Paris, Musée d’Orsay) and his Les Raboteurs de parquet, petit version (Image 1n; private collection); panels C and D are Paul Cézanne’s Cour de ferme à Auvers (“Auvers farmyard,” Image 6c; Paris, M usée d’Orsay) and his La M aison du pendu (“Hanged man’s house,” Im age 6n; Paris, Musée d’Or- say); panels E and F are Edgar Degas’ Femmes à la terrase d’un café (“Women at the café,” Image 8c; Paris, Musée du Louvre, Départment des Arts Graphiques, Fonds du M usée d’Or- say) and his L’Absinthe (“The absinthe drinker,” Image 8b; Paris, Musée d’Orsay); and pan- els G and H are Edouard M anet’s Le Balcon (“The balcony,” Im age 18c; Paris, Musée d’Or- say) and his Le Déjeuner à l’atelier (“Studio luncheon,” Image 18n; Munich, Neue Pinakothek). See the Appendix for further information. Panels A, C, D, E, F, and G are reprinted with per- m ission of the Musée d’Orsay; panel B is reprinted with permission of Bridgeman Art; and panel H is reprinted with perm ission of the Neue Pinakothek. A B C D E F G H MERE EXPOSURE HELPS MAINTAIN A CANON OF ART 321 the Musée du Luxembourg in Paris in 1897, moved to the Musée du Louvre in 1927, then to the Musée du Jeu de Paume in 1947, and f inally to the Musée d’Orsay when it opened in 1986. Given its division of the collection, did the French gov- ernment select well? According to some, very well: “With the glaring exception of Cézanne, it is arguable that [the French state] wound up with the cream of the collection” (Varnedoe, 1987, p. 202). Perhaps, but the splitting of the Caillebotte collection also offers a superb natural ex- periment in canon formation and fosters other questions. Nonetheless, the major focus of this article is on another issue. CANONS, CULTURE, AND MERE EXPOSUR E An artistic canon consists of a culture’s esteemed works of painting, sculpture, architecture, music, theater, poetry, literature, or film. The membership of works within a canon is graded—some are central, some less central but f irmly within, some on the margins, and some clearly outside. Thus, canons have the structure of any natural category (Rosch, 1973). I assume that artworks within a canon deserve their position but also that many works on the fringes and even well outside are equally worthy and equally deserving of cultural reverence. Why are these not within the canon? I would claim that a major force in canon formation is historical accident, but I haven’t the space here to defend such an idea; canon formation has been discussed at length in the humanities (e.g., Guillory, 1990, 1993; Sassoon, 2001; Hallberg, 1984). Instead, I focus here on a different issue: canon maintenance over time through its broad cultural reception. A force that helps maintain an artistic canon, I believe, is the cultural generalization of the laboratory phenom- enon called mere exposure (Zajonc, 1968, 1980), the nonconscious acquisition of information about, and atti- tudes toward, objects and events through their repeated presence in our lives. These occurrences help shape our preferences. Mere exposure is a phenomenon related to learning without awareness, or implicit learning (e.g., Roediger, 1990; Schacter, 1987; Seamon et al., 1995; Squire, 1992), but with a focus on the affective compo- nent. In particular, from childhood through college and throughout adulthood, we are exposed to hundreds of thousands of images. Some are representations of art; others, as during a museum visit, are the artworks them- selves. We do not typically remember each occurrence of each image or where we saw it. We often will not even recognize it if we see it again, but its trace can influence our future assessments. Such assessments are not overt cognitive responses on our part. They are not directly re- lated to the formal part of our education, but they are very much a part of our general and higher education. The effects of mere exposure are quite automatic and independent of what we pay attention to in our day-to- day activities. They accrue simply as the result of being a member of a culture, experiencing cultural artifacts (see Zajonc, 1970). Laboratory evidence suggests that what we are exposed to, and then prefer, can be quite meaningless in a larger context (Bornstein, 1989)—for example, line drawings, polygons, ideographs, nonsense words or syllables, or sounds. Nonetheless, they can also be meaningful—for example, photographs of objects or people. So why not paintings? Laboratory results here are mixed. Berlyne (1970) found that there were mere exposure effects for abstract paintings but that complex ones were preferred over simpler ones. Zajonc, Shaver, Tavris, and van Kreveld (1972) found the reverse effect for abstract paintings and their parts. And Brickman, Redfield, Harrison, and Crandall (1972) found both ef- fects, depending on initial responses, favorable or unfa- vorable. But as studies relying on laboratory exposure, these research efforts were not concerned with everyday exposure to artwork. The major purpose of this article, then, is to circumvent this problem—to assess effects of mere exposure as measured in a more ecological way.4 GOALS These six studies had three goals. The first two are in- tertwined and were spun from the arguments above. The f irst concerns Caillebotte. It is sometimes suggested that he had extraordinary taste in amassing his collection (e.g., Bernac, 1895/1966). Unequivocally, some of these works are among the centerpieces of the Impressionist canon. To be sure, in many cases, he was on the scene as the f irst possible buyer of particular artworks, and he seems to have exercised his preferences in an optimal sit- uation to help each friend. However, it should also be noted that many of the paintings he purchased were thought unsellable at the time he acquired them (Bazin, 1958; Rewald, 1946). Can one determine whether or not Caillebotte demonstrated exquisite taste? Through the use of paired comparisons—a major method of art his- torical analysis—one might have the opportunity to ad- dress such a difficult question. The second goal was to assess the role of the Musée d’Orsay in the maintenance of the Impressionist canon. Thus, we can ask with re- spect to contemporary viewers: Do the paintings in its collections have a special place within the Impressionist canon? If so, why? The third goal is more pertinent to experimental and social psychology and quite incidental to both Caille- botte and the Orsay. Its focus is mere exposure as in- dexed by the frequency with which the images appear in print. That is, the relative frequencies of these images in books will serve as a cultural proxy; their differential number should mimic the differential likelihood that in- dividuals will have seen these images before. The more often the images appear, the more likely it is that indi- viduals may have seen them at least once, perhaps more. This idea is essentially the same as that proposed by Fer- guson (1999), a museum curator: Every act of writing or curatorial practice, whenever it gets to the point of naming a name, is participating in a certain level of canon formation, no matter what the intent 322 CUTTING of its author, no matter whether it represents a challenge to the status quo or a confirmation of it. (p. 4) This might be modified only by adding that every act of publishing or broadcasting an image is a participation in the maintenance of, or in the change in, a canon. So I set out to enumerate particular “acts of writing or curatorial practice,” needing only to choose which images to look for and a reasonable place to count them. THE IMAGES, THEIR FREQUENCIE S, AND THEIR REPRESENTATIVENESS Image Choice and Stimulus Preparation For the centennial of Caillebotte’s death, Anne Distel, later head of the Musée d’Orsay, oversaw a celebration of his work. In Distel (1994), there are small, black-and- white images of 65 works from the Caillebotte collec- tion; 8 others are mentioned in brief detail without im- ages and, sometimes, without names. Of these 73, 4 were not considered for this study—one drawing and one wa- tercolor by Jean-François Millet, one drawing by Paul Gavarni, and one decorative fan by Camille Pissarro. From the remaining 69, at least one reasonable-quality version of 64 of the images was located—62 in books and 2 on the Internet.5 In addition, two paintings by Caillebotte himself that were eventually included in the legacy were also used. Thus, 66 images from the Caillebotte collection were used as stimuli: 2 Caillebottes, 5 Cézannes, 8 Degas, 4 Manets, 16 Monets, 14 Pissarros, 9 Renoirs, and 8 Sis- leys. These images are listed in the left column of the Appendix with their French titles and the most descrip- tive of their English titles. Also included are the dates they were painted (if known), their current locations (if known), and their catalogue raisonné reference numbers or, when not available, another reference citation. Among images for each artist, works are listed in their catalogue raisonné order, which is generally taken to be chrono- logically accurate.6 In all, 39 of these images are from the Orsay, 10 in other museums, and 17 in private col- lections or in locations unknown. To begin, I sought many high-quality color reproduc- tions of each image; 51 were found in color, 44 in at least three different printings. These were inspected for appar- ent consistency of reproduction, and what appeared to be the best image (or in the case of the other 7, sometimes the only image) was digitized on a LaCie Scanner III flatbed scanner. The resulting Photoshop 3.0 f iles were about 2 MB in size. Such resolution approximates that of 35-mm f ilm.7 These f iles were stored as JPEG files at medium resolution. The 15 remaining images were digi- tized in black and white (grayscale) at the same resolution. Each image in Caillebotte’s collection was then matched to another image, selected with several constraints: The paired image must have been by the same artist, in the same general style, from roughly the same period (me- dian difference = 2 years, SD = 3.7 years), and with the same type of subject matter. The idea is that these matched images are ones that Caillebotte might have collected had he had the opportunity, and in many cases he proba- bly did. The 66 comparison images were selected from the same general sources as those for the Caillebotte im- ages and were screened for reproduction quality in the same way. The right panels of Figures 1 and 2 show eight examples. Comparison images were chosen without regard for their location—currently in the Musée d’Or- say, another museum, or a private collection. These are listed in the right column of the Appendix. These images were digitized at the same resolution as their Caillebotte counterparts. If matched to a black-and-white image, they too were digitized in grayscale. In all, 33 of the comparison images were from Orsay, 27 from other mu- seums, and 6 from private collections or in locations unknown. In addition, among the 66 pairs, there were 16 direct comparisons of images from the Orsay, 5 of im- ages from other museums, and 1 of images in private col- lections. When not referred to by title, the paintings will be dis- cussed by image numbers, 1–66, with the additional no- tation of c (for the Caillebotte images) and n (for their matched pairs, the non-Caillebotte images). Thus, Image 26c is Monet’s La Gare Saint-Lazare (“The Saint- Lazare train station,” Figure 2A), and Image 8n is Degas’ L’Absinthe (“The absinthe drinker,” Figure 1F). Table 1 presents these image pair numbers and an abbreviated or descriptive form of their English titles for data presenta- tion and discussion. Determination of Image Frequency After I selected the images, a research assistant and I became suff iciently familiar with all 132 to recognize them spontaneously. Carrying copies of the images for reference, we then began to consult all the relevant books we could f ind in the Cornell Fine Arts Library and, later, 12 other Cornell campus libraries. Our intent was to record every occurrence of each of the 132 images in Cornell’s more than 6 million volumes. Carrying an elec- tronic notebook with us, we created and then continually updated separate databases for each of the 132 images. These were Microsoft Excel files that registered each oc- currence with its source’s call number, author, title, date of publication, page number, and occasionally, other in- formation. We found target images in books along shelves by call number according to (1) each of the eight artists, which included monographs, exhibition catalogs, cata- logues raisonnés, and works in sections on painting, sculp- ture, pastels, drawings, watercolors, prints, and com- bined media; (2) all other artists closely or even loosely associated with Impressionism, including those gener- ally painting earlier, such as Corot, Courbet, and Turner, those painting at the same time, such as Fantin-Latour and Forain, and those painting later, such as Gauguin, Seurat, Toulouse-Lautrec, and Van Gogh; (3) artistic terms, such as Impressionism, Neoimpressionism, and Postim- pressionism, but also the Nabis, Naturalism, Japonisme, Cubism, Symbolism, and more generally, 19th century MERE EXPOSURE HELPS MAINTAIN A CANON OF ART 323 Figure 2. A second group of eight Impressionist images. Again Caillebotte owned the four on the left, and again, these 4 pairs were among the 66 pairs used in these studies. Panels A a nd B are Cla ude M onet’s La Gare S aint-La zare (“T he S aint-Lazare tra in sta tion,” Image 26c; Paris, Musée d’Orsay) and his La Gare Saint-Lazare, l’arrivée d’un train (“The arrival of a train at the Saint-Lazare train station,” Im age 26n; Cambridge, MA, Fogg Mu- seum of Art); panels C and D are Camille Pissarro’s Les Toits rouges, coin du village, effet d’hiver (“Red roofs,” Im age 43c; Paris, M usée d’Orsay) and his La Côte des Boeufs à Pon- toise (Im age 26n; London, National Gallery); panels E and F are Pierre-Auguste Renoir’s Bal du M oulin de la Galette (Im age 54c; Paris, Musée d’Orsay) and his Le Déjeuner des canotiers (“Luncheon at the boating party,” Image 54n; Washington, DC, Phillips Collection); and p anels G and H are Alfred S isley ’s Les Régates à M olesey (“ The regatta at M olesey,” Image 59c; Paris, M usée d’Orsay) and his Les Régates à Hampton Court (Image 59n; Zurich, Stiftung Sammlung Emil G. Bührle). Again, see the Appendix for m ore information. Panels A, C, E, and G are reprinted with permission of the Musée d’Orsay, Paris, France; panel B is reprinted with permission of the Harvard University Art Museums; panel D is reprinted with permission of the National Gallery, London; panel F is reprinted with perm ission of the Phillips Collection, Washington, DC; and panel G is reprinted with permission of the Samm- lung E. G. Bührle, Zurich, Switzerland. A B C D E F G H 324 CUTTING T ab le 1 D es cr ip ti ve I m ag e T it le s fo r M em b er s o f E ac h P ai r E n u m er at ed A lp h ab et ic a ll y b y P ai n te r, I m ag e F re q u en ci es , R ec og n it io n R at es in S tu d ie s 1 a n d 2 ( S 1 a n d S 2) , C om p a ra ti ve C o m p le xi ty J u d gm en ts in S tu d y 4 ( S 4) , P ro to ty p ic a li ty R a ti n g s in S tu d y 5 ( S 5) , a n d P re fe re n ce s in S tu d ie s 1, 2 , 3 , 4 , a n d 6 ( S 1– S 4, S 6) Ju d g m en ts f o r th e M o re P ro to - P ro to - F re q ue n t Im ag e C ai ll eb o tt e ty pi - ty p i- C o m - C ol le ct io n F re q u en cy R ec og n it io n ca li ty C o m p ar is o n F re q u en cy R ec og n it io n ca li ty p le x it y P re fe re n ce Im ag es A ll G en er al S 1 S 2 (S 5 ) Im ag es A ll G en er al S 1 S 2 (S 5 ) (S 4 ) S 1 S 2 S 3 S 4 S 6 C ai ll eb o tt e 1 . T h re e fl o or s tr ip p er s* 6 1 3 .0 2 .1 6 2 .2 T w o fl o o r st ri p pe rs † 10 0 .0 1 .3 7 2 .0 .8 1 7 7 5 7 – 67 41 2 . S n ow -c ov er ed r o o fs * 2 6 0 .0 1 – 4 .0 R ue H al év y, 7 th f lo o r† 11 0 .0 4 – 4 .0 – 2 1 – – – 15 C éz an n e 3 . C o up le s by p on d 1 3 0 .0 4 – 4 .6 T h e pi cn ic 12 0 .0 3 – 4 .2 – 7 3 – – – 59 4 . F o u r b at he rs 3 4 0 .0 2 .0 5 3 .2 F iv e b at h er s* 17 0 .0 1 .0 5 4 .5 .6 8 7 6 6 2 – 89 73 5 . F lo w er s, r o co co v as e 1 5 0 .0 1 – 2 .1 F lo w er s, D el ft v as e* 25 0 .0 4 – 3 .9 – 6 2 – 39 – 59 6 . A uv er s fa rm y ar d * 3 0 1 .0 1 .2 1 4 .5 H an g ed m an ’s h o u se * 1 65 1 6 .0 1 .1 6 5 .0 .2 1 6 5 7 6 28 88 63 7 . B ay o f E st aq ue * 8 6 3 .0 3 .1 6 5 .4 E st aq u e lo o k in g e as t 27 0 .0 2 .1 1 5 .0 .5 0 8 7 5 8 – 82 75 D eg as 8 . W om en a t a ca fé * 8 9 6 .0 2 .4 7 4 .0 A b si n th e d ri n k er * 1 73 3 4 .0 7 .1 1 4 .5 .1 9 6 6 7 4 52 62 55 9 . T h ea te r ch o ru s* 1 8 0 .0 4 – 4 .9 D an ce r w it h b o u q u et * 37 4 .0 8 – 4 .9 – 4 6 – – – 40 10 . W om an l ea vi n g b at h * 2 9 0 .0 4 – 4 .7 W o m an d ry in g f ee t* 25 1 .0 5 – 4 .1 – 2 0 – – – 23 11 . D an ce l es so n , v io li n 2 7 0 .0 6 .2 6 4 .8 T w o d an ce rs a t b ar 44 2 .0 9 .1 6 5 .0 .3 6 5 6 6 3 – 69 53 12 . B al le t st ar * 8 7 13 .1 4 .4 2 5 .1 A ra be sq u e, b ou q u et * 23 3 .0 9 .5 3 4 .9 .1 1 7 3 4 1 – 63 48 13 . S q u at ti n g w om an * 1 4 0 .0 1 .1 1 5 .0 W o m an i n t u b * 96 1 0 .0 7 .4 0 4 .0 .8 4 7 0 8 4 – 76 67 14 . S p an is h d an ce r (b /w )* 8 0 .0 1 – 3 .9 A m b as sa d o rs c af é (b /w )* 12 0 .0 1 – 4 .2 – 6 9 – – – – 15 . D an ce r ru b b in g a n k le * 2 6 0 .1 3 – 4 .9 D an ce r, u m br el la 32 1 .0 7 – 4 .5 – 6 0 – 66 – 58 M an et 16 . A ng el in a* 1 8 0 .0 1 .0 5 2 .5 G eo rg es C le m en ce au 47 0 .0 2 .0 5 2 .3 .1 7 6 1 6 3 – 41 43 17 . T h e ra ce s (b /w )† 6 0 .0 1 – 2 .5 L on gc ha m ps h or se ra ce ( b/ w ) 11 0 .0 0 – 3 .6 – 5 6 – – – – 18 . T h re e o n a b al co ny * 2 0 1 21 .0 2 .1 5 3 .0 L u nc h in t he s tu di o 1 04 4 .0 0 .0 5 1 .9 .0 9 5 6 4 7 68 48 43 19 . C ro q u et m at ch † 9 0 .0 6 .1 6 3 .7 O n th e b ea ch 31 0 .0 5 .0 5 3 .0 .8 6 7 2 6 8 65 69 66 M o n et 20 . M t. R ib o u de t (b /w )† 3 0 .0 1 – 4 .2 V ie w , A rg en te ui l pl ai n (b /w )* 8 0 .0 0 – 4 .5 – 7 1 – – – – 21 . A rg en te u il r eg at ta * 6 6 6 .0 3 .1 6 5 .4 S ai li n g , G en n ev il li er s 24 0 .0 1 .0 5 5 .1 .6 4 4 4 5 3 – 23 33 22 . G ar d en l u n ch * 6 4 3 .0 3 .1 6 5 .1 W o m en i n g ar d en * 1 62 2 7 .0 4 .1 1 4 .0 .3 6 6 3 5 7 58 67 65 23 . A pa rt m en t an d c h il d* 3 2 0 .0 1 – 3 .5 C or n er o f a st u d io * 14 0 .0 1 – 2 .1 – 5 6 – 57 – 26 24 . T u il er ie s at d aw n * 1 5 0 .0 2 – 5 .7 S ei n e ic e br ea k- up 11 0 .0 2 – 4 .9 – 6 2 – 6 – 28 25 . G en n ev il li er s p la in 4 0 .0 0 – 5 .6 V ét h eu il , L av ac o u rt * 9 0 .0 6 – 6 .4 – 6 5 – 26 – 77 26 . S t. L az ar e st at io n * 1 2 7 21 .0 5 .2 1 5 .6 S t. L az ar e tr ai n 62 1 2 .0 2 .3 2 5 .3 .6 4 4 9 3 2 – 35 40 27 . P o nt d e R o m e, L az ar e† 1 4 0 .0 0 – 5 .5 P on t E u ro p e, L az ar e 25 2 .0 2 – 6 .3 – 7 5 – – – 66 28 . S t. L az ar e si g n al 2 0 0 .0 0 – 4 .8 S t. L az ar e tr ac ks † 10 0 .0 1 – 4 .9 – 6 7 – 68 – 56 29 . A pp le t re es ( b /w )† 7 0 .0 0 – 4 .4 V ét h eu il l an ds ca p e (b /w )* 5 0 .0 2 – 4 .3 – 4 9 – – – – 30 . V ét h eu il c hu rc h * 3 0 0 .0 1 – 4 .3 V ét h eu il w in te r 7 0 .0 0 – 5 .1 – 4 4 – – – 63 31 . P lu m t re es i n b lo o m ( b /w )† 3 0 .0 0 – 5 .1 U n d er l il ac s (b /w )* 14 0 .0 0 – 4 .3 – 7 4 – – – – 32 . H oa rf ro st * 1 7 0 .0 5 .5 2 6 .0 V ét h eu il i n m is t 23 0 .0 8 .2 2 6 .1 .3 3 7 0 7 8 65 70 59 33 . R ed m u m s (b /w )† 4 0 .0 0 – 4 .7 M u m s (b /w )* 17 0 .0 4 – 4 .3 – 7 4 – – – – 34 . S ei n e at R o ch e G u yo n ( b /w )† 3 0 .0 1 – 5 .5 S ei n e w it h ic e- fl o es ( b /w )* 32 0 .0 1 – 5 .4 – 7 5 – – – – 35 . B el le -I sl e ro ck s* 2 7 0 .0 1 – 5 .4 B el le -I sl e st o rm * 24 0 .0 3 – 4 .6 – 6 3 – 69 – 61 MERE EXPOSURE HELPS MAINTAIN A CANON OF ART 325 T ab le 1 ( C o n ti n u ed ) Ju d g m en ts f o r th e M o re P ro to - P ro to - F re q ue n t Im ag e C ai ll eb o tt e ty pi - ty p i- C o m - C ol le ct io n F re q u en cy R ec og n it io n ca li ty C o m p ar is o n F re q u en cy R ec og n it io n ca li ty p le x it y P re fe re n ce Im ag es A ll G en er al S 1 S 2 (S 5 ) Im ag es A ll G en er al S 1 S 2 (S 5 ) (S 4 ) S 1 S 2 S 3 S 4 S 6 P is sa rr o 36 . L o u ve ci en n es r o ad † 8 0 .0 1 .0 0 3 .1 E nt ra n ce t o V oi si n s* 65 1 1 .0 0 .0 5 3 .8 .4 1 7 3 6 8 61 65 73 37 . B o ug iv al w as h ho u se * 2 1 1 .0 0 – 3 .9 B an k s o f th e O is e 7 0 .0 0 – 5 .1 – 3 7 – – – 39 38 . M o n tf ou ca u lt r o ck s† 7 0 .0 0 – 5 .8 H er m it ag e h il l* 12 0 .0 0 – 4 .9 – 6 0 – 26 – 39 39 . P lo w m an † 5 0 .0 1 – 4 .5 C ha p o nv al l an d sc ap e* 21 1 .0 1 – 5 .5 – 4 4 – – – 49 40 . P o nt o is e g ar d en ( b /w )† 3 0 .0 1 – 4 .5 H er m it ag e, P o n to is e (b /w )† 2 0 .0 0 – 4 .6 – 5 6 – – – – 41 . M o n tf ou ca u lt h ar ve st * 3 3 0 .0 4 .3 2 4 .9 H ay st ac k , P o n to is e† 13 0 .0 7 .1 1 4 .5 .5 3 3 6 2 1 – 38 27 42 . A ut u m n w oo d s‡ 4 0 .0 0 .0 5 5 .9 L it tl e b ri d g e, P on to is e 13 1 .0 2 .0 5 4 .6 .1 2 6 9 3 2 – 37 28 43 . R ed r o o fs , H er m it ag e* 9 9 23 .0 1 .0 0 4 .7 C ôt e d es B o eu fs 38 4 .0 0 .0 0 4 .5 .1 3 4 2 4 2 – 38 46 44 . P o nt o is e o rc h ar d * 5 6 4 .0 1 – 5 .6 L o uv ec ie n n es o rc h ar d 23 1 .0 1 – 4 .5 – 5 7 – – – 35 45 . R ye f ie ld s, P o n to is e† 4 0 .0 6 – 5 .7 C ow he rd , P o n to is e 9 1 .0 2 – 5 .6 – 6 0 – 33 – 48 46 . P at h t h ro u g h w o od s* 1 3 0 .0 1 – 4 .9 R es ti n g i n w o o d s 8 0 .0 1 – 5 .6 – 6 2 – 28 – 45 47 . E d g e of w o od s (b /w ) 3 0 .0 0 – 4 .8 P in k h o u se ( b /w )* 6 0 .0 0 – 5 .4 – 7 2 – – – – 48 . P at h c li m b in g f ie ld s* 7 0 .0 1 – 5 .5 W o m an , E ra g ny f ie ld * 29 2 .0 4 – 5 .6 – 5 2 – – – 47 49 . W h ee lb ar ro w * 1 7 1 .0 0 – 5 .4 A u tu m n p o n d † 3 0 .0 1 – 4 .5 – 6 0 – 22 – 27 R en o ir 50 . G ir l re ad in g * 4 5 2 .0 5 .1 1 5 .7 M ad am e D ar ra s* 8 0 .0 2 .2 2 3 .2 .7 9 8 1 7 9 – 77 59 51 . S t. G eo rg es P la ce ( b /w )† 4 0 .0 0 – 4 .9 A ra b fe st iv al , A lg ie rs ( b /w )* 21 0 .0 1 – 4 .7 – 6 7 – – – – 52 . T o rs o i n s u n li g h t* 9 0 6 .0 3 .1 1 5 .6 A n n a, n u d e 29 2 .0 3 .1 6 4 .2 .6 3 5 1 5 8 – 68 44 53 . T h e sw in g * 9 4 7 .0 5 .4 2 5 .4 T h e bo w er , G al et te 18 0 .0 7 .2 1 5 .8 .4 4 7 2 5 3 – 79 66 54 . M o u li n G al et te p ar ty * 2 8 2 51 .2 5 .5 8 5 .8 B oa ti n g l u n ch eo n 1 92 3 5 .2 1 .5 8 5 .3 .8 5 5 4 4 7 34 52 40 55 . S ei n e at C h am p ro sa y * 2 1 0 .0 0 – 6 .1 S n ow y l an ds ca p e 7 0 .0 1 – 6 .0 – 4 4 – 92 – 25 56 . R ai lr o ad b ri d g e* 1 5 0 .0 1 – 5 .6 W o m an i n g ar d en 7 0 .0 8 – 6 .3 – 3 0 – – – 46 57 . C h at ea u o f m is t (b /w ) 2 0 .0 1 – 4 .6 W in d in g p at h ( b /w )* 44 0 .0 1 – 4 .4 – 6 6 – – – – 58 . G ir ls a t p ia no † ‡ (* + 4 1 ) 6 0 .0 4 – 2 .7 G ir ls a t p ia n o i n re d 19 0 .0 2 – 2 .9 – 5 1 – 33 – 38 S is le y 59 . B o at s at M o le se y * 4 1 1 .0 0 .0 5 5 .1 H am p to n C o ur t B o at s 11 0 .0 4 .1 6 5 .6 .8 6 5 0 3 2 – 40 41 60 . L o u ve ci en n es s tr ee t 7 0 .0 1 – 5 .7 S ab lo n s w o o d s* 10 4 .0 1 – 5 .2 – 4 6 – 88 – 57 61 . S ei n e at S ur es n es * 1 3 0 .0 1 – 5 .5 B ri d g e at S èv re s 2 0 .0 5 – 6 .2 – 4 5 – 71 – 41 62 . S t. M am m ès f ar m * 1 5 0 .0 5 .0 5 5 .4 V o is in s v il la g e* 12 1 .0 5 .2 1 5 .6 .4 7 7 0 5 3 74 75 61 63 . E d g e of f o re st ( b /w )* 9 1 .0 0 – 5 .3 R es ti n g b y a b ro o k ( b /w )* 9 0 .0 1 – 4 .4 – § 4 5 § – – – – 64 . S ei n e at S t. M am m ès ( b /w )* 9 0 .0 1 – 5 .7 L o in g c an al ( b /w )* 4 0 .0 3 – 5 .3 – 8 1 – – – – 65 . S ei n e at B il la n co u rt ( b/ w )† 2 0 .0 1 – 4 .5 S ei n e at B o u gi va l (b /w )* 13 0 .0 0 – 5 .1 – 9 2 – – – – 66 . S ei n e su n se t (b /w )† 2 0 .0 1 – 5 .3 B ou g iv al l oc k ( b /w )* 14 0 .0 1 – 4 .5 – 2 9 – – – – N o te — F re q u en cy , f re qu en cy c o u nt s o f ea ch i m ag e as s ea rc h ed f o r in t h e C o rn el l L ib ra ri es ; re co g n it io n , t h e p ro b ab il it y o f se lf -r ep o rt ed r ec og n it io n o f th e im ag e; p ro to ty p ic al it y, t h e ju d g m en t o f ho w t y p - ic al e ac h i m ag e is o f Im p re ss io ni st a rt ( 1 – 7 s ca le , 7 = m o st ); a ll , nu m er ic al f re q u en cy o f im ag e ac ro ss a ll l ib ra ry b o o k s; g en er al , n u m er ic al f re qu en cy i n i n tr o d u ct o ry a rt h is to ry t ex tb o o k s, w or ld i m ag e so ur ce bo o k s, e n cy cl o p ed ia s, a n d d ic ti o n ar ie s; c o m p le x it y, p ro ba b il it y t ha t th e m o re f re q ue n t im ag e of t h e pa ir w as a ls o j u d g ed a s m or e co m p le x; p re fe re n ce , th e p er ce n ta g e th at t h e m o re f re q ue n t im ag e w as l ik ed b et te r; b /w , bl ac k a n d w hi te c om pa ri so n s, c o lo r im ag es u n av ai la bl e fo r C ai ll eb o tt e im ag es . * Im ag es i n t h e M u sé e d ’O rs ay o r M u sé e d u L o u v re , fo n d s d u M u sé e d ’O rs ay . † Im ag es i n p ri va te co ll ec ti o n s, o r in lo ca ti o n s u nk n ow n . ‡ R en o ir p ai n te d s ev er al J eu n es f il le s a u p ia n o, a n d th ey a re v ir tu al ly in d is ti n g u is h ab le . C ai ll eb ot te ’s i m ag e is s li g h tl y d ar ke r in c o lo r an d h as a s li g h tl y d if fe re nt h an d po si ti o n fo r th e g ir l n o t p la y in g t h e p ia no . B ec au se o f th ei r st ri k in g si m il ar it y, t h e fr eq u en ci es f o r th e O rs ay v er si o n w er e ad d ed t o t h o se o f th e C ai ll eb o tt e v er si o n i n f re q u en cy a n al y se s. § W h en f re qu en - ci es a re t ie d , th e p er ce n ta g e p re fe re nc e o f th e C ai ll eb o tt e im ag e is g iv en . 326 CUTTING art, French art, European art, and modern art; (4) paint- ing and drawing techniques and the use of color; (5) landscapes, seascapes, portraits, still lifes, and flowers; (6) feminism, nudes, bodies, fashion, visual culture, and modern life; (7) aesthetics and form; (8) art appreciation and the psychology and philosophy of art; (9) pictorial art as it relates to music, poetry, and the other arts; (10) guidebooks to Paris and the French patrimoine, a reflec- tion of French culture, geography, and history; (11) books on art collectors and collecting; (12) books on museums and museum design; (13) source books of museum hold- ings around the world and their guide books; (14) books of images for sale as posters; (15) auction house catalogs; (16) introductory art history textbooks; (17) world art textbooks and sourcebooks; (18) encyclopedias and dic- tionaries specific to art; and (19) general encyclopedias. Later, it will be useful to distinguish among image ap- pearances in three types of books: (1) the most focused texts featuring one of the eight artists; (2–15) the broader, more topical texts featuring many artists or those featur- ing one artist but using works of another (e.g., a book on Cézanne with an image by Pissarro); and (16–19) gen- eral textbooks, sourcebooks, encyclopedias, and dictio- naries, whose purpose it is to cover all of art across all of time. In this process, we attended to shelf lists and the on-line catalog, recalled books when necessary, obtained missing books on interlibrary loan, and perhaps most im- portant—and certainly most delightful—simply roamed the aisles of the Cornell Fine Arts and other libraries in search of candidate volumes. We thumbed through as many as were deemed even remotely likely to have any of these images. Several constraints governed our tallies, following the spirit of counting the different “acts of writing or cura- torial practice” (Ferguson, 1999): Multiple copies of the same book were not considered (these were intended for the same audience, had the same call number, and were published at the same time), although a foreign language book and its English language translation were counted separately, as well as different editions of the same book (these were intended for different audiences, had differ- ent call numbers, and were published at different times). Occasionally, in a given book, there would be both a full image and a detail of it. Both of these were counted, with the idea being that if the author wished to show the image twice (or more), it was important to count it twice (or more). In the databases, we recorded the number of details for each image and the number of times the image appeared in color (vs. black and white), on covers, as frontispieces, or spread out on two pages versus one. None of these factors had any statistical leverage in the results that follow, so they will not be considered again. We searched books—sometimes intensively, some- times more leisurely—over the course of 20 months in at least 200 library visits. After several months, totals were accumulated each month or so and were compared, and correlations were computed. These were always ex- tremely high (rs > .99), and increasingly so as time pro- gressed. Thus, however many books we might have missed in assembling our databases, image counts that included them would not have changed the shape of the relationships among the frequencies reported here. In all, we located 4,232 reproductions of the 132 im- ages in 980 different books. Multiple volume sets were counted as a single book. Publication dates were between 1901 and 2002. In this effort, possibly 6,000 books were examined. Frequencies of image occurrence ranged from 2 (1 Pissarro, 1 Renoir, and 3 Sisleys) to 282 (Renoir’s Bal du Moulin de la Galette, Image 54c and Figure 2E).8 Mean frequency for all the images was 32.1; median frequency was 16. Distributions varied widely: Eight images oc- curred more than 100 times (including Figures 1D, 1F, 1G, 1H, 2A, 2E, and 2F), 13 from 50 to 100 times (in- cluding Figures 1A, 1E, 2B, and 2C), 27 from 25 to 49 times (including Figures 1C, 2D, and 2G), 44 from 10 to 24 times (including Figures 1B and 2H), and 40 fewer than 10 times. Thus, among the 132 images, there is a clear gradation from the high canon of Impressionism to its base corpus. Raw frequencies for the images in all the texts and in general texts are shown in Table 1, but a dis- cussion of them will be interlaced with the results of Study 1. An important question arises about the utility of these frequencies: How representative are the relative occur- rences of these 132 images to what would be more broadly available in our culture? This is not an easy question to resolve. Nonetheless, one can approximate an answer in two ways. The f irst related question is: How representative are the numbers of books on each artist in the Cornell collections to all the books and other sources on these painters. This was assessed in three ways. On the Representativeness of the Cornell Collections First, in April 2001, the on-line catalog of the Cornell libraries was searched for all occurrences of the last names of eight artists, used as keywords. Individual records were inspected for relevancy, and the number was recorded.9 The same was then done on line for the Bibliography of the History of Art (BHA), a professional database of books and articles since 1973 that can be searched by the same keyword technique. The correla- tion between the frequencies of the relevant titles in the Cornell collections and those in the BHA was very high (r = .97, p < .001). Relative counts are shown in Table 2. This suggests that the holdings at Cornell on these artists and the scholarly work done on them in the last third of the 20th century are quite tightly related. Given the assump- tion that the artworks of each artist should be distributed roughly in the same way in both corpora (Cornell’s and the BHA’s), Cornell’s collection seems representative of the professional literature. Second, an on-line search was conducted of current and out-of-print books on Amazon.com, screened in the same manner as before. The results are also shown in Table 2, and the correlation between the Cornell hold- MERE EXPOSURE HELPS MAINTAIN A CANON OF ART 327 ings and the Amazon listings is reasonably high (r = .80, p < .02). Differences are not many. The Amazon offer- ings overrepresented Monet and underrepresented Cézanne, as compared with the Cornell holdings and the BHA listings. Third, Internet explorations were conducted in April 2002, with the search engine Google. Search was done three ways: f irst, using the last name of the painter only, then the name in conjunction with “art”; and then the name plus “Impressionism.” The results are shown in Table 2. None of these was reliably correlated with the frequencies in the Cornell collections or in the BHA (rs < .36, n.s.). Monet and Renoir were overrepresented, and Cézanne and Manet were underrepresented, as com- pared with the Cornell collections and the BHA. On the Representativeness of the Images The second methodological question concerns the re- lation of the stimuli used here to the Impressionist canon. To this end, an index of obtained versus expected (O/E) values was created for the images of each painter. Its numerator for each artist is the total number of Cor- nell books in which any of his stimulus images appeared. This is the obtained value, O (column 8 of Table 2). The denominator is the expected value, E. This is the total number of Cornell books accessed using the ar tist’s name as a keyword and screened for content (column 2 of Table 2), plus the number of books accessed using the open-ended keyword “Impression$.” In the latter count, books on Postimpressionism were excluded, and the residual was checked for relevant content and for the presence of many images.10 These totaled 91. The logic of the index is as follows: If the artist’s works among these stimuli were central to his oeuvre, one might rea- sonably expect that at least one of them would appear in every book on the painter and in every book on Impres- sionism generally. In this manner, if the stimuli were rep- resentative, index values (O/E ) should be near 1.0. The results are shown in the column 9 of Table 2. As one can see, Renoir’s index (2.04) was more than twice what might be expected. This is in good measure due to his Bal du Moulin de la Galette, part of the Caillebotte legacy. His index would be nearer unity (1.43) without it. Three other indices were reasonably above 1.0: those for Pissarro (1.46), Degas (1.34), and Monet (1.28). Those of Manet and Cézanne were less, partly because the numbers of their paintings used here were fewer than those for the other artists. In addition, Manet’s was less (0.93) also because of extensive literatures on his Déjeu- ner sur l’herbe (1863), his Olympia (1863; both in the Orsay), his Le Bar aux Folies-Bergère (1882; Courtauld Institute, London), and works on Spanish influences and topics in his art. Except for the Folies-Bergère, Caille- botte’s collection of Manet’s work, and images here matched to them, was subsequent to these. In a reverse manner, Cézanne’s index (0.69) was considerably lower, because Caillebotte’s collection was prior to many more important works, such as the near-Cubist series of La Montagne Sainte-Victoire. Indeed, these are subsequent to Caillebotte’s death. Finally, Sisley’s index was lower still (0.56) for at least two reasons. First, his works are increasingly excluded from presentations of Impression- ism, and second, his images from the more celebrated se- ries on the floods at Marly (e.g., L’Inondation à Port- Marly in the Orsay) were not included here. The fact that indices for these seven artists are generally greater than 1.0 (mean index = 1.19), coupled with the high correla- tion of works in the Cornell collections with those in the BHA database, suggests that these stimuli form a suit- able and representative sample of the Impressionist canon. Table 2 also shows two related indices in the last two columns: that for the representativeness of the Caille- botte images for each painter to the Impressionist canon and that for those images matched to them. Across artists, the means of these indices were 0.77 for both the Table 2 For Each Painter, the Number of Sources Found in Electronic Searches, the Number of Books in the Cornell University (CU) Libraries With Their Im ages, and the Indices of the Representativeness of their Images in the Entire Stimulus Set, Those in the Caillebotte Collection, and Those of the Im ages Matched to Them Number of BooksOn-Line Searches in CU Indices of Academic and Google (31,000) Libraries Obtained/Expected Popular Sources Painter With Frequencies CU Painter +“Impres- These Caille- Comparison Artist Libraries BHA* Amazon Painter +“Art” sionism” Images All† botte Images Caillebotte 16 , 90 8 8.8 5.0 1.6 61 – – – Cézanne 243 1,200 83 116.0 47.2 8.7 230 0.69 0.39 0.53 Degas 156 ,853 119 131.0 62.0 14.3 330 1.34 0.70 1.09 Manet 168 ,942 84 155.0 48.2 9.3 240 0.93 0.68 0.52 Monet 156 ,800 130 846.0 230.0 25.2 316 1.28 0.79 0.89 Pissarro 60 ,414 27 41.6 20.3 5.8 221 1.46 0.95 0.92 Renoir 115 ,372 73 298.0 105.0 13.0 420 2.04 1.46 1.11 Sisley 33 , 77 8 153.0 14.2 3.5 70 0.56 0.38 0.32 *BHA is the Bibliography of the History of Art. †The obtained variable is the number of books in the Cornell libraries with any of the selected images by each painter (column 8). The expected variable is the number of books found in the on-line catalog, using the painter’s name (column 2), plus the 91 books in the catalog on Impressionism, screened for content. 328 CUTTING Caillebotte images and their comparisons, suggesting that the two groups are well matched in representing each artist’s work. STUDY 1 Frequency Counts, Recognition, and Preferences for Images From the Impressionist Canon and Corpus Using the frequency counts of the images as an esti- mate of the likelihood of prior exposure, I set out to de- termine whether there was a relationship between the counts and viewers’ preferences for the images and be- tween the counts and viewer recognition. Also of inter- est were the relative preferences for, and recognition of, the Caillebotte and Orsay images. M ethod All JPEG files were commercially made into 5 3 5 cm slides. Two carousel trays were prepared, with members of each pair in corresponding slots— one image from the Caillebotte collection in one tray and one matched to it in the other. Their order in sequence was random, with several constraints: No images by the same artist could follow one another; black-and-white pairs could not follow each other; half of the Caillebotte images appeared on the left, and half appeared on the right; and 31 images from the Musée d’Orsay appeared on the left, and 41 on the right. Stimulus pairs were presented for about 8 sec each. They were projected onto a large screen by two projectors. The smaller di- mension of each image (vertical if landscape, horizontal if portrait) projected a size of about 8º from the middle of the auditorium. Viewers were 166 Cornell University undergraduates enrolled in a perception course. They were asked to look at each pair and judge which image they liked best. In addition, should they recognize any image, they were to mark which of the two, or both, they recognized of each pair. No record was kept of where the viewers sat. Previous research using a wide variety of stimuli seen from a wide variety of locations in such situations has shown no effect (Cutting, Wang, Flückiger, & Baumberger, 1999; Gibson, 1947; Kozlowski & Cut- ting, 1977). The viewers also filled out a brief questionnaire, indicating how many times they had visited the Johnson Art Museum on the Cor- nell University campus each year (54% said at least once a year), the number of times they had visited any other museum (52% said at least once a year), the number of times they had ever visited the Musée d’Orsay (8% had visited at least once), and how many art history courses they had taken (only 16% had taken any). The stu- dents also reported spending a mean of 10.8 hours per week on the Internet (median = 10, SD = 9.3). Results and Discussion The results divide several ways. In increasing order of psychological interest, f irst the results pertinent to the Caillebotte collection and to that of the Musée d’Orsay and then those interrelating recognition, preference, and frequency will be considered. Caillebotte and the Orsay. Across the database, im- ages from the Caillebotte collection did not appear with any different frequency than their matched pairs. Means were 33.5 (SEm = 5.8) and 30.6 (SEm = 5.1, n.s.), respec- tively. In addition, the Caillebotte images were not claimed to be recognized with any reliably different frequency— 2.5% versus 2.9% (n.s.), respectively. And f inally, the viewers expressed no preference for the Caillebotte im- ages, choosing them 47.5% of the time (vs. 52.5%, n.s.). Thus, there is nothing unusual here about the paintings and pastels in the Caillebotte collection. He cannot be said to have had extraordinary taste in the selection of his images over and above the selection of other images by groups of other collectors—at least for these com- parisons, as judged by a contemporary, relatively naive, but appreciative artistic audience. Next, consider the Musée d’Orsay. Images in its col- lection appeared significantly more often than those elsewhere. Means were 43.1 (SEm = 6.0) versus 20.9 (SEm = 3.2, p < .0001), respectively. This difference oc- curred even when the comparison was restricted only to those images in all the other museums (43.1 vs. 27.5, p < .005). This is not a surprise, since the French govern- ment and the Réunion des Musées Nationaux of France have been thorough in promoting their art for a long time. However, the viewers did not claim to recognize the images in the Orsay more often than others—2.8% versus 2.6% (n.s.), respectively. As might be expected, viewers did prefer them somewhat more often—54% versus 46% [r = .32; t(38) = 2.14, p < .04]—but when frequency differences were factored out, there was no ef- fect of images being in the Orsay (t < 1, n.s.). Thus, what distinguishes this selection of the Orsay holdings is only that its images appear more often. What about Varnedoe’s (1987, p. 202) claim that the state of France got the “cream” of the Caillebotte col- lection? Of course, an empirical analysis of the kind here carries a different force than an academic or professional assessment. Yet one must be wary of how this retrospec- tive prophecy may have been fulfilled; certain of Caille- botte’s images went to the state of France, which made them available and promoted them, which we now re- vere. It is unequivocal that the Caillebotte images in the Orsay appear more often than those that are not (means of 49.7 vs. 9.6, p < .0001).11 And indeed, the Caillebotte Orsay images were somewhat preferred over their matched pairs (53% to 47%, n.s.), whereas those not in the Orsay were not (42% vs. 58%, p < .01). But once the difference in relative frequency is factored out [r = .50; t (63) = 4.08, p < .0002], there was no residual effect of images being in the Orsay (r = .24; t < 1, n.s.). Thus, being in the Orsay does not make a painting part of the canon inde- pendently of how often it appears. Instead, an image may appear more often because it hangs in the Orsay (and hung in its predecessor museums), and appearing there often goes some distance toward maintaining an artwork in the canon as acknowledged by professionals and the public. Finally, paintings and pastels that reside in any museum— the Orsay or elsewhere—appeared reliably more often than those in private collections (37.4% vs. 5.6%; p < .0001), they were recognized more often (3.3% vs. 1.5%; p < .0001), and when paired directly they were preferred more often (61% vs. 39%; p < .001). None of this, of MERE EXPOSURE HELPS MAINTAIN A CANON OF ART 329 course, is a surprise. Essentially, by definition, artworks in private collections cannot be in a canon. What drives all of this, at least statistically, would appear to be fre- quency of appearance. The other relationships among frequency, recognition, and preference are the center- piece of the f indings in this study and are a bit complex. Recognition and frequency. Viewers claimed to rec- ognize only 2.7% of all the images, but this varied ac- cording to observer experience, as is shown in the top panel of Figure 3. Those having never taken an art his- tory course, or having taken only one, recognized 1.6% of all images, and those having had at least two courses recognized 11.9% ( p < .001). Those claiming not to go to an art museum each year recognized 1.1% of the im- ages, whereas those claiming to go at least once a year recognized 4.1% ( p < .001). Finally, those never having been to the Musée d’Orsay recognized 1.9%, whereas those having been at least once recognized 13.4% ( p < .001). Multiple regression showed that the number of im- ages viewers claimed to recognize was correlated with how often they went to any museum (r = .49, p < .0001) and how often they had been to the Musée d’Orsay (r = .49, p < .0001), but once these were factored out, not with how many art history courses they had taken (r = .35, n.s.). The f irst two factors accounted for 39% of the variance in recognition judgments across viewers (R = .62, p < .0001). Recognition of individual images ranged from 0% (2 Manets, 9 Monets, 10 Pissarros, 2 Renoirs, and 3 Sisleys) to 25.4% (Renoir’s Bal du Moulin de la Galette, Image 54c and Figure 2E). Rates for each of the 132 images are given in Table 1. The general patterns will be discussed here. Analyses were then conducted predicting viewers’ recognition of all the images from three different sets of frequencies: (1) those from general texts, (2) those from more topical texts with narrower scope but not featuring a single artist, and (3) those from more focused texts and monographs featuring a single artist. The counts from the first group seem the most f irmly in the Impression- ist canon, those from topical books a bit less so, and those from more focused texts less firmly still, leaving those in none of the books except the catalogues raison- nés in the broad base of the corpus. Thus, in moving from high canon to corpus, it is best to compare the fre- quencies of images in (1) the most general texts with (1+2) those in the general group plus the topical texts, with (1+2+3) those in all texts. These can be conceived as different slices through likely cultural exposure. Figure 3. The contrasts between recognition and preference results as a function of observer sophistication and experience in Study 1. Standard errors are shown. .16 .12 .08 .04 .60 .55 .50 N Y Y N Y N Y1 2+ N YN YN Y Took An Art History Course? Visited a Museum in the Past Year? Ever Visited the Musée d’Orsay? P ro b ab il it y of P re fe rr in g th e M or e F re q u en t Im ag e P ro b ab il it y of R ec og n it io n n = 14 n = 137 n = 13 n = 77 n = 87 n = 151 n = 13 330 CUTTING Consider a hypothetical example: If an image ap- peared in five general texts, 15 topical texts, and 25 mono- graphs, the numerical values for the three classes would be 5, 20, and 45. Psychophysical practice suggests that these should be logarithmically transformed. Since zeros occur (most images never appear in general texts) and since the log of zero is undefined, the natural log (n + 1) was used, where n is the value under consideration (More- land & Zajonc, 1977). Thus, the values above would be log (5 + 1), log (20 + 1), and log (45 + 1), or 1.79, 3.04, and 3.83, respectively. Since such measures are always correlated, multiple regression is the best statistical tool for titrating effects of the appearances of images in the different types of academic sources. Claims of image recognition were most correlated with their log frequency of occurrence in general art texts [r = .46; F(1,128) = 7.5, p < .01], but not with the sum of the general and topical texts [r = .41; F(1,128) < 1, n.s.] or with occurrences across all texts [r = .43; F(1,128) = 1.8, n.s.]. The multiple correlation accounted for 23% of the variance in the data [R = .48; F(3,128) = 12.0, p < .0001], but clearly, almost all of the effect came from the frequencies of images in the most general sources. This result is not a surprise. Those images should be the most recognizable. However, in this con- text, it is important to note that there is currently no in- troductory art history course at Cornell and that only 27 of 166 (16%) undergraduates had taken any art history courses. Thus, I claim that recognition reflects general knowledge of particular viewers, not their perusal of in- troductory art history textbooks or encyclopedias in the Cornell libraries or elsewhere. Most of the image pairs were in color, but 15 were black and white. Did this effect recognition? Yes. That is, when the differential frequencies were factored out, there remained an effect of color: Color images were claimed to be recognized 3.7% of the time, but black- and-white images only 0.7% [r = .31; F(1,127) = 9.52, p < .003].12 The most frequently occurring black-and- white stimulus was Image 57n. If comparison color im- ages are restricted to its value (n = 44) and less, the dif- ference is still reliable (2.8% vs. 0.7%, p < .01). Color also improves recognition of objects and scenes in labo- ratory studies (Joseph & Proff itt, 1996; Oliva & Schyns, 2000; Wurm, Legge, Isenberg, & Luebker, 1993). Note that no assumption is made that the observers’ responses necessarily represent the true recognition of a particular painting or pastel. There is no way to verify them. Nonetheless, there are additional interesting trends. For example, against a backdrop recognition of 2.7%, the 16 images by Degas were recognized at a rate of 6.1% ( p < .001), and the 7 of his images that were clearly dancers were recognized at a mean rate of 9.4%. Recog- nition of the dancer images, it would seem, is an exam- ple of generic recognition—recognition by that individ- ual only that he or she had seen images of Degas-like dancers before (for a laboratory analogue, see Monahan, Murphy, & Zajonc, 2000). Given that there are 600 pas- tels and paintings of dancers in the Degas catalogue raisonné (Lemoisne, 1946), this is perhaps not entirely surprising. Preference and frequency. Herein lie the key results of the study. Over all pairs, the viewers preferred the more frequently occurring image of each pair on 59% of all the trials. This highly reliable effect [t(165) = 9.09, p < .0001] is about the size of many mere exposure ef- fects in the literature (e.g., Seamon & Delgado, 1999). Indeed, 48 of the more frequent images in 64 pairs were preferred (with one tie in preference and one in fre- quency; z = 3.9, p < .001). Unlike the recognition results, this effect was uniform across all types of observers, as is shown in the bottom panel of Figure 3. It occurred equally for those who never had had an art history course [59%; t (130) = 8.14, p < .0001] and those who had taken at least one such course [59%; t(27) = 4.01, p < .0001]. It occurred for those not visiting a museum in the past year [59%; t(79) = 5.1, p < .0001], those visiting once [59%; t(46) = 6.8, p < .0001], and those visiting at least twice [58%; t (38) = 5.4, p < .0001], and it occurred equally for those not visiting and those visiting the Musée d’Orsay (59% each, ps < .001). Importantly, when differences in recognition rates were compared with preferences for each of the 66 images pairs, there are no reliable correlation (r = .18, n.s.). Bornstein (1989), among others, noted that although mere exposure effects are a function of number of expo- sures, the effect asymptotes with increasing numbers of presentations. Preferences were then compared against the difference in log values of the images for the three levels of texts in which they appeared. Again, if one image appeared in f ive encyclopedias, 15 topical texts, and 25 monographs, the natural log transformation of the frequencies of 6, 21, and 46 would be 1.79, 3.04, and 3.83, respectively. If its comparison image appeared in 0, 25, and 40 such texts, the log of 1, 26, and 66 would be 0.0, 3.25, and 4.19. Differences in log values would then be 1.79, 20.21, and 20.36. These values would be used as inputs to the regression. Multiple regression analysis showed that viewer pref- erence was not correlated with the difference in log fre- quency of image appearance in the most general texts (r = .36, F < 1, n.s.) or with the differences in the sum of frequencies in the f irst group and the topical texts (r = .37, F < 1, n.s.). However, it was reliably correlated with the difference in frequencies in all occurrences [r = .54; F(1,62) = 11.5, p < .001]. The multiple correlation on preference results accounted for 30% of the variance in the data [R = .55; F(3,62) = 9.1, p < .001], again with most of the effect coming from a single source—this time, differences in overall frequency. Overall frequen- cies for each image, I claim, act as a proxy for the like- lihood and frequency with which an individual may have been exposed to that image in his or her broader cultural experience. Interestingly, the correlation between pref- erences and the differences in occurrences in all books (r = .54) is the same regardless of whether all the occur- rences of the images since 1901 are considered, or only those since 1989. The latter, of course, are likely the oc- MERE EXPOSURE HELPS MAINTAIN A CANON OF ART 331 currences correlated with what is most relevant to these viewers. This result also speaks strongly to the stability of the canon of images across the course of the 20th cen- tury, a topic to which I will return in the conclusion. A variety of other frequency transforms were correlated with preference results, but none provided as strong a correlation as log (n + 1). Given this result, the greater the difference in the logarithms, the stronger the prefer- ence should be. Indeed, this is what occurred. Figure 4 shows the relative preferences for the more frequent image across three groups of image pairs. The increasing trend seen in the f igure is what drives statistically reliability [r = .54; t(64)= 5.1, p < .0001]. Unlike recognition responses, preferences were not af- fected by color versus black-and-white presentation. That is, once the differences in relative frequencies of the various types of books are factored out, there is no re- maining contribution of color [F(1,61) < 1, n.s.]. Nonethe- less, without considerations of frequency, the more fre- quent image in a black-and-white pair was actually preferred more often (67%) than was the more frequent image of a color pair [57%; t(62) = 2.37, p < .01]. This would appear to be due to variance in small numbers: The generally lower frequencies of occurrence for black- and-white pairs generated greater mean difference ratios (4.9:1) than for color pairs (2.8:1). In addition, all digital f iles were inspected for mean lu- minance and RGB values. Preference within pairs was not related to differences in luminance (r = .11, p > .35) or to distributions of red (r = 2.04), green (r = 2.05), or blue (r = .03) colors. Indeed, the multiple correlation of all these variables was also not significant (R = .09, p > .20). STUDY 2 Preference and Recognition in an Older Group The goals of this second study were to replicate the primary results of Study 1 with an older group. In par- ticular, the f irst study showed preferences related to dif- ferences in frequencies of occurrence for pairs of images and no relation between preference and recognition. This joint result is important for demonstrating mere expo- sure. Nonetheless, the lack of relation could be due, in part, to a floor effect, because so few of the images were claimed to be recognized. An older group would likely recognize more. Method Twenty-five pairs of images were selected from the 66 pairs used in Study 1. These are indicated in Table 1—1 Caillebotte, 3 Cézannes, 4 Degas, 3 Manets, 4 Monets, 4 Pissarros, 4 Renoirs, and 2 Sisleys. These were among the more frequent in the database of images from all books (mean frequency of 61.0 vs. overall mean of 32.1 in Study 1). They were also among the more recognized (mean of 4.8% vs. 2.7% in Study 1). No black-and-white pairs were used in this study. To vary presentation format, digital images were mounted in pairs on the same slide as part of a PowerPoint presen- tation. The left–right arrangement of half the pairs was reversed from that of Study 1, and the presentation order was also changed, but otherwise with the same constraints. In all, 13 Caillebotte im- ages appeared in the left, 12 on the right; 14 Orsay images were on the left, 16 on the right. For most of the viewers, the largest dimen- sion of each image subtended about 8º. As a group, 9 faculty mem- bers (mean age = 46 years) and 10 graduate students (mean age = 25 years) viewed the 25-pair sequence; 12 had visited the Musée d’Orsay at least once. Each made preference judgments and marked which images they remembered having seen before. Results and Discussion This group reported recognizing 18.6% of the images, many more than in Study 1 [t(47) = 7.8, p < .0001]. Indi- vidual image rates are given in Table 1. Claims of recog- nition were unrelated to frequencies of appearance in all books (t < 1, n.s.) and in general tests [t(47) = 1.2, n.s.] and were unrelated to their preferences [r = 2.31; t(23) = 1.6, n.s.]. There was no difference in recognition of Caillebotte versus comparison images and, independent of frequency, no superior recognition of Orsay images (ts < 1, n.s.). Nonetheless, as in Study 1, the viewers preferred the more frequent image of each pair—here, 57% of the time [t(23) = 3.6, p < .002]; 17 of 25 pairs showed this effect (z = 2.2, p < .02). Rates for each comparison are given in Table 1. This set of images had about the same prefer- ence margin in Study 1 (58% vs. 42%). In addition, there was a high correlation of preferences among the 25 image pairs used in the two studies (r = .84, p < .0001). Again, preferences were related to the log difference in frequencies across all books [r = .57; F(1,22) = 4.7, p < .05], but less so to the log difference in frequencies in general texts (r = .43, F < 1). Independent of frequency, however, there was no preference for Caillebotte images or Orsay images (ts < 1, n.s.). Thus, the major features of the results of Study 1 were replicated. STUDY 3 Preferences of Children The goal of this study was to determine whether the pattern of preference results found in Studies 1 and 2 Figure 4. Difference in the ratio of overall frequency of the im- ages in the pairs and the mean preference of the more frequent of the pair in Study 1. In general, images were preferred over their m ates as a function of how much more often they occurred. 332 CUTTING might be found in a group of children. Should children’s preferences match those of previous studies, something other than mere exposure must be at work; children sim- ply lack broad exposure to art. Here, I also wanted to focus on a group of images that would be least likely to have been seen before. M ethod Twenty-four different pairs of images were selected from the 66 pairs. These are indicated in Table 1: 2 Cézannes, 2 Degas, 2 Manets, 7 Monets, 5 Pissarros, 3 Renoirs, and 3 Sisleys. Nineteen pairs contained images that were among the least frequent from all the books— mean image frequency of 15.0 versus 32.1 in Study 1. These will be called rare images. For comparison purposes, f ive pairs containing the most frequent images were also included— Image Pairs 6, 8, 18, 22, and 54, with a mean frequency of 146 per image. These will be called common images. Mean Study 1 prefer- ence rates for the more frequent images were 59% for both the rare and the common pairs. Nine of the 24 pairs had been used in Study 2; 5 were common-image pairs. Again, no black-and-white pairs were used in this study. Again, digital images were mounted in pairs on the same slide as part of a PowerPoint presentation. The left–right arrangement was balanced and randomized for the Caille- botte images and for the more frequent images, and the presentation order was randomized anew. Otherwise, the same constraints were followed as those in Study 1. Sixty-three students in a Montessori school participated in three groups as part of an art class. There were 13, 28, 19, and 10 stu- dents, whose ages were 6, 7, 8, and 9 years old, respectively. In ad- dition 5 teachers participated. Each made preference judgments on an answer sheet. Particular care was taken that all the students un- derstood the instructions. Pairs were presented for a minimum of 10 sec each. Questions were allowed during the tests, so presenta- tion rates varied. Results and Discussion These children showed no preference for the Caille- botte (49.5% vs. 50.5%) or Orsay (45.2% vs. 54.8%; ts < 1, n.s.) images. More important, they also showed no preference for the more frequent image of each pair— 51.2% versus 48.8% [t (22) = .26, p > .75]. Common (49.4% vs. 51.6%) and rare (52.0% vs. 48.0%) pairs did not differ in this regard. Interestingly, mean teacher pref- erence was 60% for the more frequent image of each pair [t(22) = 4.1, p < .06]. The children’s responses, however, were far from random; they seemed to like paintings with brighter colors. Responses of each age group correlated re- liably with those of all other age groups [rs > .63; ts(22) > 3.9, ps < .001], but they did not correlate with the pref- erences of the adults of Study 1 for the same images (r = 2.16, p > .40). Overall preferences for more frequent images were 48.7%, 47.1%, 53.0%, and 55.8%, respec- tively, for the 6-, 7-, 8-, and 9-year-old groups. Nonethe- less, the variance was large, and the apparent increasing trend was not reliable, either for all the pairs [t(61) = 1.56, p > .12] or for the rare pairs [t(61) = 1.6, p > .10]. Thus, whatever governed the adult preferences in Studies 1 and 2 is not operative in the preferences of chil- dren. This is an important null result. Although elemen- tary school children may have seen a few Impressionist paintings before, they lack the broad cultural exposure to Impressionist art that adults have experienced. STUDY 4 Complexity Is Not a Mediator of Preference The goals of this fourth study were twofold: to replicate the preference results of Studies 1 and 2 and to explore the possible contributions of what is called stimulus complexity. Bornstein (1989) reviewed the literature on the relation between complexity and preference in labo- ratory experiments on mere exposure. He found that six of nine published studies found stronger mere exposure effects for complex stimuli than for simple stimuli, but more relevant are those using art works as stimuli. Here, the results are mixed. As was mentioned earlier, Berlyne (1970) and Zajonc et al. (1972) found opposing effects: one favoring a preference for complex stimuli, the other for those simpler. Nonetheless, the results of neither of these studies are generally applicable to representational art. Both confined themselves to abstract art (where bits and pieces could be counted in the context of complex- ity), and the latter carved up the artworks (under the as- sumption that a detail is less complex than the whole). Since complexity cannot be defined with rigor in many domains (Goodman, 1972), I let the observers define it for themselves. Method The viewers were 112 students from a classroom population, dif- ferent from those in Study 1, but taking the same class a year later. Only 17% had taken an art history course, and 7% had been to the Musée d’Orsay. The same 25 pairs were used as those in Study 2, again in a PowerPoint presentation, but shown in a different order and with some counterbalanced changes in left– right positions. Again, pairs were presented for about 8 sec each. Viewers were asked to make a judgment about which image in each pair they pre- ferred. In addition, after the first run through of the stimulus set, the pairs were presented a second time, and the viewers were asked to make judgments about which image was more complex. Results and Discussion Again, from the database tallies of all the books, the more frequent image of each pair was preferred over its counterpart [58% vs. 42%; t(24) = 16.7, p < .0001], the same preference margin for these images as in Study 2. Again, 17/25 pairs followed this pattern (z = 2.2, p < .02), but 4 pairs changed polarity of preference. Overall, preferences here were well correlated with those for the same images in Study 1 (r = .80, p < .0001). Individual pair results are again shown in Table 1. The correlation of preference with frequency differences across all the books was reliable and about the same as that in Study 1 [r = .47; F(1,22) = 6.3, p < .02], and again, that for gen- eral texts was not [r = .21; F(1,22) = 1.0, n.s.]. Finally, the Caillebotte images were not preferred over their matched pairs (48% vs. 52%, n.s.), Orsay images were not preferred either (49% vs. 51%, n.s.), and preferences were unrelated to museum trips or art history courses taken. Complexity judgments were not correlated with pref- erences (r = .18; t < 1, n.s.). Individual means for each image are shown in Table 1. Inspecting the relative judg- MERE EXPOSURE HELPS MAINTAIN A CANON OF ART 333 ments for each pair suggests that viewers were simply counting things, usually people, in the pictures. Consider four image pairs exhibiting strongly judged complexity differences. Degas’s Femmes à la terrase d’un café, le soir (“Women outside a café in the evening”) was judged as more complex than his L’Absinthe 81% of the time (Image Pair 8, Figures 1E and 1F). This may be because there are f ive people in the foreground of the former (with many in the background) and only two in the lat- ter. Renoir’s Bal du Moulin de la Galette was judged more complex than his Le Déjeuner des canotiers (“Lun- cheon of the boating party;” Image Pair 54, Figures 2E and 2F) by 85% of the viewers. This is likely because the former has uncountably many people and the latter but a dozen. Pissar ro’s La Côte des Boeufs à Pontoise was judged as more complex than his Les Toits rouges (“The red roofs”) 87% of the time (Image Pair 43, Figures 1D and 1C). Both are views of the Hermitage in Pontoise in winter, but the former has more trunks of tall trees in the foreground. Finally, Manet’s Le Déjeuner à l’atelier (“Lunch in the studio;” with three people) was judged more complex than his Le Balcon (with three people clearly visible and a fourth in shadow; Image Pair 18, Figures 1H and 1G) 91% of the time. This is probably because the table in the former is cluttered with food, and the scene is cluttered with other objects. There is no such clutter in the latter. STUDY 5 Prototypicality is Not a Mediator of Preference Another account for the preference results of the Stud- ies 1, 2, and 4 might be that viewers, when faced with making preference judgments, were comparing images on the basis of what they thought were the most repre- sentative (prototypical) Impressionist paintings. This study was designed to address this issue. M ethod Twenty-one undergraduate students in an advanced visual percep- tion seminar viewed a PowerPoint sequence of 138 images—all 132 used in Study 1, plus 6 more by Gustave Caillebotte. 13 These images were presented singly, and viewers rated them on a scale of 1 to 7 as to how representative each was of Impressionist paintings, with 7 being the most prototypical . Presentation was haphazard, with the constraints that images by the same artist could not follow one an- other, nor could black-and-whit e images. Six students had participated in either Study 1 or 4, and 6 had taken at least one art history course. Results and Discussion Prototypicality judgments were not correlated with the experimental variables of previous interest—the fre- quencies of the images in all the texts or in the introduc- tory texts (rs = 2.05 and .06, respectively, n.s.) or the recognition rates in Studies 1 and 2 (rs < .20, n.s.). The differences in prototypicality judgments within a pair were also not correlated with preferences in Studies 1, 2, or 3 (rs < .14, n.s.) or complexity judgments in Study 4 (r = 2.05, n.s.). Prototypicality judgments for individual images are shown in Table 1. These null results may seem odd. After all, the most frequently occurring im- ages are the prototypes of the canon. Nonetheless, it seems that the viewers were making judgments in a dif- ferent way. There were some striking effects of prototypicality judgments by painter. Most prototypic were the 16 im- ages of Sisley (mean rating = 5.28). This is interesting, because he is clearly the least major of the seven “major” Impressionists, as suggested by the book counts in Table 2. Clustered next and together were the 32 works of Monet (4.96), the 28 of Pissarro (4.93), and the 18 of Renoir (4.96), with the f irst two reliably different from Sisley ( ps < .05). Clustered next, and reliably below these four, were the 16 works of Degas (4.59) and the 10 of Cézanne (4.24). Finally, well below these were the 8 of Manet (2.81) and the 10 of Caillebotte (2.77). Inter- estingly, Cézanne and Manet are often described as not really being Impressionist painters (Cézanne’s most im- portant works are later than the period of the 1870s and 1880s, and Manet’s earlier), and Caillebotte as well. In addition, Degas never painted outdoors, which may have influenced judgments. Other classifications of images also show some inter- esting differences. Of this set of 138 images, 90 can be classified as landscapes, 44 as portraits (often of groups and often outside), and 4 as still lifes. Mean ratings for landscapes (4.96) were reliably higher than those for portraits [4.14; t(132) = 4.71, p < .0001] and still lifes [3.75; t(92) = 5.2, p < .0001]. Portraits and still lifes did not differ. It should be noted that, among these images and throughout their oeuvres, Sisley painted only land- scapes, Pissarro mostly landscapes, Renoir mostly por- traits, and Degas almost exclusively portraits. OVERVIEW Preference The adult viewers of Studies 1, 2, and 4 generally liked the more frequent images of each pair, but the chil- dren of Study 3 did not. The effect in the adults was salient for differences measured across all the books in the Cornell databases; preference was not independently related to frequency differences in general texts. Prefer- ence strength was not a function of whether or not im- ages were in color (Study 1; in Studies 2 and 4, all were in color), nor were they a function of whether or not the observers took trips to museums or attended art history courses. Recognition In Study 1, viewers recognized few of these Impres- sionist images—less than 3%. Low recognition rates are requisite for laboratory demonstrations of mere expo- sure (Kunst-Wilson & Zajonc, 1980). In that study, recog- nition rates were related to their frequency in general texts in the Cornell collections, but not to their frequency in all the books. Recognition was more frequent for color images, and it was also related to viewers’ visits to the Musée d’Orsay and other art museums. In Study 2, a 334 CUTTING more seasoned set of viewers recognized 18% of a smaller set of images, but their recognition rates were not related to any frequency counts. In Studies 1 and 2, the viewers’ recognition rates and preferences were not related, an- other requisite for laboratory demonstrations of mere ex- posure. This pattern has been recognized for quite some time (Zajonc, 1980), and there is neurophysiological ev- idence in its support (Elliott & Dolan, 1998). Art Collections Several conclusions can be drawn. First, at least with respect to this type of analysis and experiment, images from the Caillebotte collection were neither preferred more often nor recognized more often than those matched to them, and the Caillebotte images were not more fre- quently occurring. Second and in contrast, the Musée d’Orsay holdings did occur more often in this sample. This is not a surprise. A systematic culling of images from the Orsay, which owns one tenth of all Impression- ist paintings publicly available, would form the bulk of the core of the canon.14 However, with the same caveats as above, the Orsay’s holdings were neither preferred nor recognized more often than other images matched to their frequencies of occurrence. Third, art in private col- lections is not in the Impressionist canon. These images occurred less often in the literature; they were less fre- quently recognized; and, lacking exposure, they were preferred less. On the Availability of Art Together, all of these trends support the idea that it is not where an image is, or who bought it, but how often it appears that affects public appreciation. Any artwork in a prized location—such as in the Musée d’Orsay—has a great advantage over other artworks, but systematic pro- motion by other museums and authors can overcome this advantage. Preference Judgments and Artistic Quality Surely the most interesting result of these studies is the relation of viewer preferences to how often images occurred in the Cornell libraries. Mere exposure aside, how else might this effect be accounted for? In dis- cussing these results with colleagues, quite a few have suggested that perhaps viewers can simply judge quality, choosing the “better” picture. I don’t believe this for a moment, but this is not the place to deal at length with this important and thorny issue. Here, let me simply ac- knowledge that there are many statements outside the art historical literature (e.g., Kant, 1794/1952; Pirsig, 1974) about people’s culturally independent ability to judge quality, as well as many within it (e.g., Rosenberg, 1967; Woodford, 1983). Nevertheless, there are also more re- cent and culturally sophisticated counters to this claim (e.g., Bal & Bryson, 1991; Cheetham, 2002; Moxey, 1994). Indeed, even Kenneth Clark, director of the Na- tional Gallery London in his 20s and, years later, a BBC icon of the visual arts, wrote in his memoirs (Clark, 1974): At the age of nine or ten I said with perfect confidence “this is a good picture, that is a bad one” . . . This almost insane self-confidence lasted till a few years ago, and the odd thing is how many people have accepted my judge- ments. My whole life might be described as a long, harm- less confidence trick. (p. 47) Nonetheless, rather than trying here to rule out quality as a mediator of these results, let me try one last time to rule in mere exposure in a different way. This also pro- vides an experimental effect that the notion of quality cannot explain. STUDY 6 Preferences From the Combination of Cultural and Classroom Exposure If mere exposure mediates preferences for artworks, it ought to be possible to combine the effects of exposures to art across two situations—the viewers’ personal his- tories with these images outside the classroom and class- room exposure to them. Method All 51 color pairs from Study 1 were used as stimuli. During 21 class periods in an introductory perception course (a year after Study 4 and 2 years after Study 1) and at the beginning of each lec- ture, students were presented 12 (on 18 days) or 13 (on 3 days) im- ages, for a total of 255 presentations, 5 per pair. Across the 21 ses- sions and for each pair, the less frequent image from the Cornell Library tallies (see Table 1) was presented four times; the more fre- quent image was presented only once. Each image was presented singly for about 2 sec as a PowerPoint slide, without comment. Pre- viously, Harrison and Zajonc (1970), Marcus and Hakmiller (1975), and Vanbeseleare (1983) found no effects of presentation rates in the range from 2 to 50 sec; it is the presentation that matters, not its duration. Several constraints governed the composition of a day’s set of slides: Images that would later be a test pair were not both presented, and no more than two images by the same artist could ap- pear successively. More important, the same image did not appear twice on the same day or even on successive days. On a 22nd class period, the 51 pairs of images were presented side by side in a PowerPoint presentation for about 6 sec per pair in a new random sequence. Insofar as possible, three presentation fac- tors were counterbalanced, left and right— more frequent images (25 left, 26 right), Caillebotte images (26/25), and Orsay images (28/30). In addition, no pairs of images by the same painter fol- lowed one another. The delays between exposure(s) and test ranged between 12 and 84 days. Although no studies in Bornstein’s (1989) meta-analysis used delays as long as these, the general result across the literature is that short delays produce weak effects, those up to two weeks relatively strong effects, and naturalistic studies (such as those presented here) with indeterminate delays often produce the strongest effects. Indeed, Harrison (1977) showed that both heterogeneous display sequences (no successively repeating items) and longer delays enhance exposure– affect relationships. The 116 viewers in attendance were asked to indicate on a re- sponse sheet which image of each pair they liked best. They also filled out a brief questionnaire, as in Study 1. Mean self-reported attendance over the 21 exposure sessions was 18 days (median = 19, SD = 3.2); 70% said that they visited the Johnson Art Museum MERE EXPOSURE HELPS MAINTAIN A CANON OF ART 335 on the Cornell University campus at least once a year, 47% said they visited at least one other art museum each year, 14% said they had visited the Musée d’Orsay, and 22% said they had taken at least one art history course. The students also reported spending a mean of 12.5 h per week on the Internet (median = 10, SD = 9.6). For the critical comparisons, the preferences of viewers in this study were compared with those of Study 1, a similar group but without the systematic classroom exposure. Thus, major analyses here are based on a between-group design, looking at differences in preferences across the 51 image pairs. Results and Discussion More frequently published images were no longer pre- ferred, accruing only 48% of all the judgments. This was reliably lower than the 57% preference in Study 1 for these 51 colored-image pairs [F(1,50) = 26.8, p < .001]. Indeed, in 41 of 50 image pairs (with one tie), the more frequent image received a smaller proportion of prefer- ence judgments (z = 4.2, p < .0001). Preference rates for the more frequent image are found in the last column of Table 1. No other results proved statistically reliable: Image pairs that were published more frequently (sum > 50, n = 26) changed, on average, as much as those that occurred less often (sum < 51, n = 25; 9.0% vs. 9.2%, re- spectively), and image pairs whose frequency ratios were relatively large (>2.5, n = 26) changed as much as those whose ratios were nearer unity (< 2.5, n = 25; 10.1% vs. 8.1%, respectively). If observers were able to judge quality alone in the image pairs, their judgments should not have been con- taminated by appearance differences in the classroom. To be sure, quality could still play a role, but such an ac- count must then rely on two processes—mere exposure and quality assessment (however that might be done). My proposal is that these are one-process results and done on the basis of mere exposure inside and outside the classroom. CONCLUSION: M ERE EXPOSUR E IS A MEDIATOR OF CANON MAINTENA NCE How might mere exposure affect an artistic canon, its reception, and its maintenance? All of us, as members of a culture, absorb what is around us. As visual beings, we digest images voraciously, even without noticing. A very small proportion of these images are from the Impres- sionist corpus and canon. Nonetheless, we respond to their occurrences in our future interactions with Impres- sionism. We like the ones we have seen before and, par- ticularly, those we may have seen many times.15 We f ind these images everywhere. Impressionist paintings are not only in galleries, but also in books and on textbook covers, calendars, posters, coasters, tee shirts, and towels, and one can find them readily on the Internet. Mere exposure dictates that every occurrence can matter, particularly when an image is otherwise rare. Museum curators ought to note the full implications of this f inding. Museums already do a reasonably good job at promoting their collections, but placing images every- where and without cost to the public will go a long way toward ratifying the importance of their collections as re- ceived by the broader public. But the competition is stiff; everyone seems to be doing it. Currently, the correlation of what’s in the literature and what’s on the Internet, as shown in Table 2, is not high. If this difference is main- tained, the canons of the future may change in directions independent of goals and interests of art professionals. Thus, I claim that artistic canons are promoted and maintained, in part, by a diffuse but continual broadcast of their images to the public by museums, authors, and publishers. The repeated presentation of images to an au- dience without its necessarily focused awareness or re- membrance makes mere exposure a prime vehicle for canon maintenance. Tacitly and incrementally over time, this broadcast teaches the public to like the images, to prefer them, eventually to recognize them as part of the canon, and to want to see them again. In turn, it seems likely that this implicit education also reinforces the choices made by professionals in what they present to that public. The public’s appreciation rewards museums, scholars, and the publishing industry by demonstrating an interested and responsive audience. And so it goes, with mere exposure cyclically rein- forcing the canons through generations of authors and curators, on the one hand, and of museumgoers and book buyers, on the other. Although it may be tacit, I do not claim that this is necessarily a subversive trend or one to be denigrated. I claim it is part of the same force that binds a culture. It is part of our human nature, built on an evolutionary substrate that makes very good sense. It helps ensure steadiness in culture more generally and relative constancy in artistic canons more particularly. REFERENCE S Bal, M., & Bryson, N. (1991). Semiotics and art history. Art Bulletin, 73, 174-208. Bazin, G. (1958). Impressionist paintings in the Louvre (S. Cunliffe- Owen, Trans.). London: Thames & Hudson. Bérhaut, M. (1994). Gustave Caillebotte: Catalogue raisonné des peintures et pastels. Paris: La Bibliothèque des Arts. Berlyne, D. E. (1970). Novelty, complexity, and hedonic value. Per- ception & Psychophysics, 8, 279-286. Bernac, J. (1966). The Caillebotte bequest to the Luxembourg. Reprinted and translated in D. Sutton, Gustave Caillebotte, 1848–1894: A loan exhibition in aid of the Hertford British Hospital in Paris, 15th June–16th July, 1966. London: Wildenstein. (Original published in 1895) Bonnano, G. A., & Stilling, N. A. (1986). Preference, familiarity, and recognition after repeated brief exposure to random geometric shapes. American Journal of Psychology, 99, 403-415. Bornstein, R. (1989). Exposure and affect: Overview and meta-analysis of research, 1968–1987. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 265-289. Brickman, P., Redfield, J., Harrison, A. A., & Crandall, R. (1972). Drive and predisposition as factors in the attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 8, 31-44. Chastel, A. (1971). Paris. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Cheetham, M. A. (2002). Kant, art, and art history. Cambridge: Cam- bridge University Press. Clark, K. (1974). Another part of the wood. London: John Murray. Cutting, J., Wang, R. F., Flückiger, M., & Baumberger, B. (1999). Human heading judgments and object-based motion information. Vi- sion Research, 39, 1079-1105. Daulte, F. (1959). Alfred Sisley: Catalogue raisonné de l’oeuvre peint. Paris: Durand-Ruel. http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0031-5117^28^298L.279[aid=348035] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0002-9556^28^2999L.403[aid=293644] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0033-2909^28^29106L.265[aid=188552] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0042-6989^28^2939L.1079[aid=2385745] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0031-5117^28^298L.279[aid=348035] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0042-6989^28^2939L.1079[aid=2385745] 336 CUTTING Daulte, F. (1971). Auguste Renoir: Catalogue raisonné de l’oeuvre peint: Vol 1. Figures 1860-1890. Lausanne: Durand-Ruel. Distel, A. (1990). Impressionism: The first collectors. (B. Perroud- Benson, Trans.). New York: Harry N. Abrams. Distel, A. (Director) (1994). Gustave Caillebotte: 1848–1894 [Art exhibition]. Paris: Réunion des Musées Nationaux. Elliott, R., & Dolan, R. J. (1998). Neural response during preference and memory judgments for subliminally presented stimuli: A func- tional neuroimaging study. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 4697-4704. Ferguson, R. (1999). Can we still use the canon? Art Journal, 58, 4. Fezzi, E. (1972). L’opera completa di Renoir [The complete works of Renoir]. Milan: Rizzoli Editore. Fox, B. (1983, October 20). Video thrills the Hollywood star. New Sci- entist, 100, 203-208. Gibson, J. J. (1947). Motion picture testing and research (Army Air Force Aviation Psychology Research Rep. 7). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Goodman, N. (1972). Problems and projects. Indianapolis, IN: Bobbs- Merrill. Guillory, J. (1990). Canon. In F. Lentrichhia & T. McLaughlin (Eds.), Critical terms for literary study (pp. 233-262). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Guillory, J. (1993). Cultural capital: The problem with literary canon formation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Hallberg, R. v on (Ed.) (1984). Canons. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Harrison, A. A. (1977). Mere exposure. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Ad- vances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 39-83). New York: Academic Press. Harrison, A. A., & Zajonc, R. B. (1970). Effects of frequency and du- ration of exposure on response competition and affect ratings. Jour- nal of Psychology, 75, 163-169. Hayward Gallery (1985). Renoir. Catalog of the exhibit at the Hay- ward Gallery, London, 30 June–21 August 1985, Galérie Nationale de Grand Palais, Paris, 14 May–2 September 1985, Museum of Fine Arts, Boston, 9 October–5 January, 1986. Jamot, P., & Wildenstein, G. (1932). Edouard Manet (Vols. 1 and 2). Paris: Les Beaux-arts. Joseph, J. E., & Proffitt, D. R. (1996). Semantic versus perceptual in- fluences of color on object recognition. Journal of Experimental Psy- chology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 22, 407-429. Kant, I. (1952). Critique of aesthetic judgment. (J. Meredith, Trans.) Oxford: Oxford University Press, Clarendon Press. (Original work published 1794) Klinger, M. R., & Greenwald, A. (1994). Preferences need no infer- ences? The cognitive basis of unconscious mere exposure effects. In P. M. Niedenthal & S. Kitayama (Eds.), The heart’s eye: Emotional influences in perception and attention (pp. 67-85). San Diego: Aca- demic Press. Kozlowski, L. T., & Cutting, J. E. (1977). Recognizing the sex of a walker from a dynamic point-light display. Perception & Psycho- physics, 21, 575-580. Kunst-Wilson, W. R., & Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Affective discrimina- tion of stimuli that cannot be recognized. Science, 207, 557-558. Lemoisne, P. A. (1946). Degas et son oeuvre (Vols. 1– 4). Paris: Arts et Métiers Graphiques. Marcus, M. G., & Hakmiller, K. L. (1975). Effects of frequency, du- ration of study trials and total duration of exposure on affective judg- ments. Psychological Reports, 37, 195-200. Marrinan, M. (2002). Caillebotte as a professional painter: From Stu- dio to the public eye. In N. Broude (Ed.), Gustave Caillebotte and the fashioning of identity in Impressionist Paris (pp. 21-65). New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press. Mead, K. H. (Director) (1974). The Impressionists and the salon (1874–1886) [Art exhibition]. Riverside, CA: Regents of the Uni- versity of California. Monahan, J. L., Murphy, S. T., & Zajonc, R. B. (2000). Subliminal mere exposure: Specific, general, and diffuse effects. Psychological Science, 11, 462-466. Moreland, R. L., & Zajonc, R. B. (1977). Is stimulus recognition a necessary condition for the occurrence of exposure effects? Journal of Social & Personality Psychology, 35, 191-199. Moxey, K. (1994). The practice of theory: Poststructuralism, cultural politics, and art history. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. Musée d’Orsay (1990). Catalogue sommaire illustré des peintures (Vols. 1 and 2). Paris: Réunion des Musées Nationaux. Nord, P. (2000). Impressionists and politics. London: Routledge. Oliva, A., & Schyns, P. G. (2000). Diagnostic colors mediate scene recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 176-210. Pirsig, R. (1974). Zen and the art of motorcycle maintenance. New York: Morrow. Pissarro, L.-R., & Venturi, L. (1939). Camille Pissarro – son art – son oeuvre (Vols. 1 and 2). Paris: Paul Rosenberg. Rewald, J. (1946). The history of Impressionism. New York: Museum of Modern Art. Rewald, J. (1996). The paintings of Paul Cézanne: A catalogue raisonné (Vols. 1 and 2). New York: Harry N. Abrams. Roediger, H. L., III (1990). Implicit memory: Retention without re- membering. American Psychologist, 45, 231-246. Rosch, E. H. (1973). Natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 328- 350. Rosenberg, J. (1967). On quality in art: Criteria of excellence, past and present. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Saltzman, C. (1998). Portrait of Dr. Gachet. New York: Penguin. Sassoon, D. (2001). Becoming Mona Lisa. New York: Harcourt. Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cogni- tion, 13, 501-518. Seamon, J. G. , Brody, N., & Kauff, D. M. (1983). Affective discrim- ination of stimuli that are not recognized: II. Effect of delay between study and test. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 21, 187-189. Seamon, J. G., & Delgado, M. R. (1999). Recognition memory and af- fective preference for depth-rotated solid objects: Part-based struc- tural descriptions may underlie the mere exposure effect. Visual Cog- nition, 6, 145-164. Seamon, J. G., Williams, P. C., Crowley, M. J., Kim, I. J., Langer, S. A., Orne, P. J., & Wishengrad, D. L. (1995). The mere exposure effect is based on implicit memory: Effects of stimulus type, encoding con- ditions, and number of exposures on recognition and affects judg- ments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 21, 711-721. Shikes, R. E., & Harper, P. (1980). Pissarro: His life and work. New York: Horizon. Smith, E. R. (1998). Mental representation and memory. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psy- chology (4th ed., pp. 391-445). Boston: McGraw-Hill. Squire, L. R. (1992). Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis of f indings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychological Review, 99, 195-231. Vanbeseleare, N. (1983). Mere exposure: A search for an explanation. In W. Doise & S. Moscovici (Eds.), Current issues in European social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 239-278). London: Cambridge University Press. Varnedoe, K. (1987). Gustave Caillebotte. New Haven, CT: Yale Uni- versity Press. Venturi, L. (1936). Cézanne, son art, son oeuvre (Vols. 1 and 2). Paris: Paul Rosenberg. Wadley, N. (Ed.) (1987). Renoir: A retrospective. New York: Macmil- lan. White, B. E. (1984). Renoir: His life, art, and letters. New York: Harry N. Abrams. Wildenstein, D. (1974–1985). Claude Monet: Biographie et catalogue raisonné (Vols. 1–5). Paris: La Bibliothèque des Arts. Winkielman, P., Zajonc, R. B., & Schwarz, N. (1997). Subliminal affective priming resists attributional interventions. Cognition & Emotion, 11, 433-465. Woodford, S. (1983). Looking at pictures. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Wurm, L. H., Legge, G. E., Isenberg, L. M., & Luebker, A. (1993). Color improves object recognition in normal and low vision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 19, 899-911. Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 9, 1-27. http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0270-6474^28^2918L.4697[aid=211328] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0022-3980^28^2975L.163[aid=665028] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0278-7393^28^2922L.407[aid=847164] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0031-5117^28^2921L.575[aid=296578] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0036-8075^28^29207L.557[aid=213661] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0956-7976^28^2911L.462[aid=2203542] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0010-0285^28^2941L.176[aid=5185419] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0278-7393^28^2913L.501[aid=57313] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0278-7393^28^2921L.711[aid=304092] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0033-295X^28^2999L.195[aid=215396] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0269-9931^28^2911L.433[aid=259293] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0096-1523^28^2919L.899[aid=311706] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0022-3514^28^299L.1[aid=73201] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0022-3980^28^2975L.163[aid=665028] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0278-7393^28^2922L.407[aid=847164] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0031-5117^28^2921L.575[aid=296578] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0956-7976^28^2911L.462[aid=2203542] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0278-7393^28^2913L.501[aid=57313] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0278-7393^28^2921L.711[aid=304092] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0033-295X^28^2999L.195[aid=215396] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0269-9931^28^2911L.433[aid=259293] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0096-1523^28^2919L.899[aid=311706] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0022-3514^28^299L.1[aid=73201] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0096-1523^28^2919L.899[aid=311706] MERE EXPOSURE HELPS MAINTAIN A CANON OF ART 337 Zajonc, R. B. (1970, February). Brainwash: Familiarity breeds com- fort. Psychology Today, 4, 33-35, 60-62. Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no infer- ences. American Psychologist, 35, 151-175. Zajonc, R. B. (2001). Mere exposure: A gateway to the subliminal. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 224-228. Zajonc, R. B., Crandall, R., Kail, R. V., & Swap, W. (1974). Effects of extreme exposure frequencies on different affective ratings of stimuli. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 38, 667-678. Zajonc, R. B., Shaver, P., Tavris, C., & van Kreveld, D. (1972). Ex- posure, satiation, and stimulus discriminability. Journal of Personal- ity & Social Psychology, 21, 270-280. NOTES 1. Caillebotte left no records concerning the accumulation of his col- lection. He seems to have acquired 57 of the 66 artworks used here in the 7 years up to and including 1882, and probably only 9 in the 12 years thereafter. Among the latter, 3 were gifts (1 from Monet and 2 from Renoir), and 6 were purchased (3 Sisleys and 3 Manets). The Manets were purchased in 1884 at the studio sale to support his widow. Nonetheless, in 1882, Caillebotte retired from Paris and removed him- self from new developments in art. In that year, Georges Seurat had yet to paint his f irst divisionist ( pointilliste) painting; Paul Gauguin was still a stockbroker; Vincent Van Gogh, although a painter for 2 years (having given up teaching and mission work), was still 4 years away from moving to Paris; and Henri Toulouse-Lautrec was a teenage art student. 2. As a part of a larger project, I inspected 30 books on Impression- ism published over the 20th century and found that these seven artists had works appearing in at least 29 of them. The next most frequent artists were Berthe Morisot (25), Mary Cassatt (22), Georges Seurat (21), Frédéric Bazille (19), Paul Gauguin (18), Henri Toulouse-Lautrec (16), Gustave Caillebotte (14), and Vincent Van Gogh (13). 3. Much has been made of this quote, and it takes many different forms. Shikes and Harper (1980), for example, cited it as including Manet rather than Monet. 4. Most recent research on mere exposure methodologically allies it- self with subliminal perception. That is, stimuli are presented briefly and then masked so that observers cannot report what they have seen but can be shown to have processed it through results of priming or prefer- ence (e.g., Kunst-Wilson & Zajonc, 1980; Monahan, Murphy, & Za- jonc, 2000; Moreland & Zajonc, 1977; Seamon, Brody, & Kauff, 1983). These results are interesting and important, but from the perspective of this article, subliminal perception is a laboratory phenomenon used to mimic the processes in real life perception—inattention and forgetting over the long haul. Otherwise, it can have no general interest. Thus, in this context, I am less interested in alternative theories that may explain mere subliminal exposure (e.g., Bonnano & Stilling, 1986; Klinger & Greenwald, 1994; Smith, 1998; Winkielman, Zajonc, & Schwarz, 1997; Zajonc, 2001) than in the more general phenomenon itself. 5. Once found, the images not appearing in Distel (1994) were checked for their provenance (ownership history), to be sure they had been Caillebotte’s. In addition, despite 20 months search in libraries and on the Internet, I was unable to find five of these works. Mention of these appeared without images in Distel (1994) or in images in the cat- alogue raisonné of each painter. They include one Monet, three Pissar- ros, and one Sisley—all listed at the end of the Appendix. 6. A catalogue raisonné assembles and illustrates, typically chrono- logically, the entire corpus of an artist’s work, often with additional ma- terial, such as correspondence. Inevitably, it has inconsistencies and er- rors revealed by later research. In this context, dating works is a particular problem with Cézanne. Thus, Rewald’s (1996) compendium was a necessary revision of Venturi (1936). Similarly, there are dating problems with Degas. For example, Distel (1994) noted that Image 14c (Danseuse espagnol, “Spanish dancer”) was purchased by Caillebotte before May 1879, yet Lemoisne (1946) listed it as having been painted in 1880. Some catalogues raisonnés, such as that of Manet (Jamot & Wildenstein, 1932), occasionally list images thematically (e.g., Image Pair 17), rather than chronologically. 7. Fox (1983), from Kodak data and in the context of predictions about high-definition TV, reported that 35-mm film has a digital reso- lution of about 2,500 3 1,800 pixels, or 4.5 MB. The f iles used in Study 1 were about 65% of that resolution along one dimension. 8. Renoir’s Bal du Moulin de la Galette is almost surely the most re- produced of all Impressionist paintings. For example, in one Paris guidebook (Chastel, 1971), I found only two paintings representing what might be found in the city—Mona Lisa and Bal du Moulin de la Galette. In turn, the Mona Lisa (also La Joconde) is often regarded as the world’s most famous painting (Sassoon, 2001) or, if one counts Michelangelo’s ceiling of the Sistine Chapel, the second most famous (September 24, 1995, London Sunday Times, The Culture, Section 10, p. 29). However, the latter assessment was made not long after the Sis- tine ceiling was cleaned, and with understandable effects of its accom- panied media coverage. 9. Inspecting each entry is necessary: “Monet” is also one of the mid- dle names of both Lamarck and Lavoisier, as well as the Russian word for money; Pierre-Auguste Renoir must be distinguished from his younger son the filmmaker, Jean Renoir (except in the latter’s book about the former); Camille Pissarro must be separated from his sons, particularly Lucien who was also an artist (but who also wrote of his fa- ther); and there are several authors named Sisley, as well as an Italian fashion line. 10. To exclude Postimpressionism is to exclude some descriptions of the work of Cézanne and, to a lesser degree, Degas. However, these works were composed after Caillebotte stopped collecting in 1882. Pis- sarro also had a Neoimpressionist ( pointilliste) period, but this too oc- curred after Caillebotte actively collected. 11. The Orsay comparison images appeared a mean of 39.5 times and the non-Orsay comparisons 25.1 times. Note, however, than many more of the comparison images (27) were from other museums than the Caillebotte images (10). 12. There was no reliable effect of color versus black and white in the counts of images in the Cornell books that effected either recognition or preference, but the effect reported above concerns the presence of color versus black and white in the stimuli as presented to the viewers. 13. The six additional Caillebotte paintings were: Déjeuner (“Lun- cheon,” 1876, private collection), Portraits à la campagne (“Country portraits,” 1876, Musée Baron Gérard, Bayeux, France), Le pont de l’Europe (“The Europe bridge,” Paris, 1876, Musée du Petit Palais, Genève, Switzerland), Peintres en bâtiment (“House painters,” 1877, private collection), Rue de Paris; temps de pluie (“Paris street, rainy weather,” 1877, Art Institute of Chicago), and Boulevard vu d’en haut (“Boulevard viewed from above,” 1880, private collection). Reference citations for these images are Bérhaut (1994) #37, 40, 49, 53, 57, and 154, respectively. 14. This figure was determined through analysis of the catalogues raisonnés of the seven “major” Impressionists and the holdings catalog of the Musée d’Orsay (Musée d’Orsay, 1990). 15. It is often noted that the effects of mere exposure may even de- cline with experience repeated very many times (e.g., Bornstein, 1989; Zajonc, Crandall, Kail, & Swap, 1974). This may well be true in labo- ratory situations or in the real world with massed practice, but it is not clearly the case with exposures distributed over years, even decades. (Continued on next page) http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0003-066X^28^2935L.151[aid=23836] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0022-3514^28^2921L.270[aid=2203544] http://gessler.ingentaselect.com/nw=1/rpsv/cgi-bin/linker?ext=a&reqidx=/0022-3514^28^2921L.270[aid=2203544] 338 CUTTING APPENDIX The Caillebotte and Comparison Images, Their Dates (If Known), Their Locations (If Known), and Their Reference Citations From the Caillebotte Legacy and Collection Comparison Images Caillebotte 1. Les Raboteurs de parquet Les Raboteurs de parquet (petit version) “The floor strippers” “The floor strippers (small version)” 1875, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2718) 1876, private collection, Paris Bérhaut (1994) #34; Figure 1A Bérhaut (1994) #35; Figure 1B 2. Vue de toits, effet de neige Rue Halévy, vue d’un sixième étage “Snow-covered roofs in Paris” “Rue Halévy, view from the 7th floor” 1878, Musée d’Orsay (RF 876) 1878, private collection Bérhaut (1994) #96 Bérhaut (1994) #100 Cézanne 3. Au bord de l’étang or Scène champêtre Le Déjeuner sur l’herbe “Couples relaxing by a pond” or “The pond” “The picnic” or “Luncheon on the grass” 1876–1877, Museum of Fine Arts, Boston 1876–1877, Musée de l’Orangerie Venturi (1936) #232, Rewald (1996) #244 Venturi (1936) #238, Rewald (1996) #287 4. Baigneurs au repos, III Les cinq baigneurs “Bathers at rest” “The five bathers” 1876–1877, Barnes Foundation 1875–1877, Musée d’Orsay (RF 1982-42) Venturi (1936) # 276, Rewald (1996) #261 Venturi (1936) #268, Rewald (1996) #254 5. Fleurs dans un vase rococo Bouquet au petit Delft or Vase de fleurs “Flowers in a Delft vase” “Flowers in a rococo vase” 1873, Musée d’Orsay (RF 1951-33) 1876, National Gallery of Art, DC Venturi (1936) #183, Rewald (1996) #227 Venturi (1936) #222, Rewald (1996) #265 6. Cour de ferme à Auvers La Maison du pendu, Auvers-sur-Oise “Farmyard at Auvers” “House of the hanged man” 1879–1880, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2760) 1873, Musée d’Orsay (RF 1970) Venturi (1936) #326, Venturi (1936) #133, Rewald (1996) #389; Figure 1C Rewald (1996) #202; Figure 1D 7. Le Golfe de Marseille, vu de l’Estaque* La Baie de l’Estaque vue de l’est “Estaque” “The bay of Estaque looking east” 1878–1880, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2761) 1878–1879, Memorial Art Gallery, Venturi (1936) #428, Rewald (1996) #390 University of Rochester, NY Venturi (1936) #408, Rewald (1996) #394 Degas 8. Femmes à la terrase d’un café, le soir Dans un café or L’Absinthe or Un Café, boulevard Hausmann “The absinthe drinker” “Women outside a cafe in the evening” 1876, Musée d’Orsay (RF 1984) 1877, Musée d’Orsay (RF 12257) Lemoisne (1946) #393; Figure 1F Lemoisne (1946) #419; Figure 1E 9. Choristes or Les Figurants Danseuse au bouquet saluant “The chorus” or “The supernumeraries” “Dancer with bouquet, bowing” 1876–1877, Musée d’Orsay (RF 12259) ~1877, Musée d’Orsay (RF 4039) Lemoisne (1946) #420 Lemoisne (1946) #474 10. Femme sortant du bain Après le bain “Woman leaving the bath” “Nude woman drying her feet” 1876–1877, Musée d’Orsay (RF 12255) 1885–1886, Musée d’Orsay (RF 4045) Lemoisne (1946) #422 Lemoisne (1946) #874 11. La Leçon de danse Danseuses à la barre “Portrait of a dancer at her lesson” “Dancers practicing at the bar” ~1879,Metropolitan Museum of Art, New York 1876–1877, Metropolitan Museum of Art, New York Lemoisne (1946) #450 Lemoisne (1946) #408 12. L’Étoile (Danseuse sur la scène) Danseuse tenant un bouquet à la main “Ballet (The star)” “Arabesque” 1876–1878, Musée d’Orsay (RF 12258) ~1877, Musée d’Orsay (RF 4039) Lemoisne (1946) #491 Lemoisne (1946) #418 MERE EXPOSURE HELPS MAINTAIN A CANON OF ART 339 APPENDIX (Continued) 13. Femme nue, accroupie, de dos Le Tub “Squatting woman seen from the back” “Woman bathing in a shallow tub” ~1879, Musée d’Orsay (RF 12254) 1886, Musée d’Orsay (RF 4046) Lemoisne (1946) #547 Lemoisne (1946) #872 14. Danseuse espagnol Le Café-concert or Chanteuse de café-concert “At the Café des Ambassadeurs” “Study for the bust of a ballet dancer” 1885, Musée d’Orsay (RF 4041) 1880, Musée d’Orsay (RF 12260) Lemoisne (1946) #814 Lemoisne (1946) #608 15. Danseuse nouant son brodequin L’Attente “Seated dancer kneading her ankle” “Dancer and woman with a black umbrella, waiting” 1881–1883, Musée d’Orsay (RF 12271) ~1882, Getty Museum, Malibu, CA Lemoisne (1946) #658 Lemoisne (1946) #698 Manet 16. Angelina Georges Clemençeau “Angelina” “Georges Clemençeau” 1865, Musée d’Orsay (RF 3664) 1879–1880, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2641) Jamot and Wildenstein (1932) #118 Jamot and Wildenstein (1932) #372 17. Les Courses† Courses à Longchamps† “The races” “At the races” 1865, location unknown 1875, National Gallery, Washington Jamot and Wildenstein (1932) #204 Jamot and Wildenstein (1932) #205 18. Le Balcon Le Déjeuner à l’atelier “The balcony” “Luncheon in the studio” 1868–1869, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2772) 1868, Neue Pinakothek, Munich Jamot and Wildenstein (1932) #150; Figure 1G Jamot and Wildenstein (1932) #149; Figure 1H 19. Croquet à Boulogne Plage avec personnages or La Partie de croquet “On the beach at Boulogne” “The croquet game” 1869, Virginia Museum of Art, Richmond 1868–1871, Woll family, private collection Jamot and Wildenstein (1932) #166 Jamot and Wildenstein (1932) #197 Monet 20. Le Mont Riboudet à Rouen au printemps Vue de plaine à Argenteuil “Mt. Riboudet at Rouen in spring” “View of the plain at Argenteuil” 1872, private collection, U.S. 1872, Musée d’Orsay (MNR 855) Wildenstein (1974–1985) #216 Wildenstein (1974–1985) #220 21. Régates à Argenteuil Voilier au Petit-Gennevilliers “Regattas at Argenteuil” “Sailboat at Petit-Gennevilliers” ~1872, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2778) 1874, Lucille Ellis Simon Collection, USA Wildenstein (1974–1985) #233 Wildenstein (1974–1985) #336 22. Le Déjeuner Femmes au jardin “Luncheon in the garden” “Women in the garden” 1873, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2774) 1866, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2773) Wildenstein (1974–1985) #285 Wildenstein (1974–1985) #67 23. Un Coin d’appartement Coin d’atelier “Apartment interior” “Corner of a studio” 1875, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2776) 1861, Musée d’Orsay (MNR 136) Wildenstein (1974–1985) #365 Wildenstein (1974–1985) #6 24. Les Tuileries (esquisse) La Débâcle “The Tuileries” “Ice break up” 1875, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2705) 1880, Museum of Art, University of Michigan Wildenstein (1974–1985) #403 Wildenstein (1974–1985) #565 25. La Plaine de Gennevilliers Vétheuil, vu de Lavacourt or La Plaine d’Argenteuil “Vétheuil, view from Lavacourt” “The plain near Gennevilliers” 1879, Musée d’Orsay (RF 1998) 1877, Fogg Art Museum, Harvard Wildenstein (1974–1985) #528 Wildenstein (1974–1985) #437 340 CUTTING APPENDIX (Continued) 26. La Gare Saint-Lazare La Gare Saint-Lazare, l‘arrivée d’un train “St. Lazare train station, Paris” “Gare Saint-Lazare, the arrival of a train” 1877, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2775) 1877, Fogg Art Museum, Harvard Wildenstein (1974–1985) #438; Figure 2A Wildenstein (1974–1985) #439; Figure 2B 27. La Gare Saint-Lazare: sous le pont Le Pont de l’Europe, gare Saint-Lazare de l’Europe “The Pont de l’Europe” “Gare Saint-Lazare: exterior view” 1877, Musée Marmottan, Paris or “Pont de Rome” Wildenstein (1974–1985) #442 1877, private collection Wildenstein (1974–1985) #447 28. La Gare Saint-Lazare à l’extérieur, le signal Les Voies à la sortie de la gare Saint-Lazare “The Gare Saint-Lazare, the signal” “The tracks in front of the Gare Saint-Lazare” 1877, Landesmuseum, Hannover 1877, private collection, Japan Wildenstein (1974–1985) #448 Wildenstein (1974–1985) #445 29. Pommiers, Vétheuil Paysage, Vétheuil “Apple trees, Vétheuil” “Landscape, Vétheuil” 1878, Wesley M. Dixon Collection, U.S. 1879, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2623) Wildenstein (1974–1985) #490 Wildenstein (1974–1985) #1986 30. L’Église de Vétheuil, neige Vétheuil l’hiver “The church at Vétheuil, snow” “Vétheuil, winter” 1878–1879, Musée d’Orsay (RF 3755) 1879, Frick Gallery, New York Wildenstein (1974–1985) #506 Wildenstein (1974–1985) #507 31. Pruniers en fleurs or Poiriers en fleurs Lilas, temps gris “Blossoming plum trees” “Resting under the lilacs” or “Blossoming pear trees” or “Lilacs, gray weather” 1879, location unknown 1972–1973, Musée d’Orsay (RF 1680) Wildenstein (1974–1985) #519 Wildenstein (1974–1985) #203 32. Le Givre Vétheuil dans le brouillard “Hoarfrost (Vétheuil)” “Vétheuil in the mist” 1880, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2706) 1879, Musée Marmottan, Paris Wildenstein (1974–1985) #555 Wildenstein (1974–1985) #518 33. Chrysanthèmes rouges Chrysanthèmes “Red chrysanthemums” “Chrysanthemums” 1880, location unknown 1878, Musée d’Orsay (RF 1951-36) Wildenstein (1974–1985) #635 Wildenstein (1974–1985) #492 34. La Seine entre Vétheuil et La Roche Guyon Les Glaçons or Débâcle sur la Seine “The Seine between Vétheuil and “The ice-floes” La Roche Guyon” 1880, Musée d’Orsay (RF 1965-10) 1881, private collection, France Wildenstein (1974–1985) #567 Wildenstein (1974–1985) #674 35. Les rochers de Belle-Île Tempêtes, Côtes de Belle-Île “The rocks of Belle-Isle” “Storm at Belle-Isle” 1886, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2777) 1886, Musée d’Orsay (RF 3163) Wildenstein (1974–1985) #1100 Wildenstein (1974–1985) #1116 Pissarro 36. Louveciennes Entrée du village de Voisins “Louveciennes” “Entrance to the village of Voisins” 1871, private collection, Paris 1872, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2456) Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #123 Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #141 37. Le Lavoir, Pontoise Pontoise, Côte de l’Oise “The washhouse at Bougival” “Pontoise, banks of the Oise” 1872, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2732) 1872, Getty Museum, Malibu Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #175 Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #182 38. Paysage avec rochers, Montfoucault Côteau de l’Hermitage, Pontoise “Landscape with rocks, Montfoucault” “Hill at the Hermitage, Pontoise” 1874, private collection, Paris 1873, Musée d’Orsay (RF 1983-8) Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #282 Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #209 MERE EXPOSURE HELPS MAINTAIN A CANON OF ART 341 APPENDIX (Continued) 39. Le Labourer Paysage à Chaponval “The plowman” “Chaponval landscape” 1876, private collection, Paris 1880, Musée d’Orsay (RF 1937–51) Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #340 Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #509 40. Jardin en fleurs, Pontoise L’Hermitage à Pontoise “Garden in bloom, Pontoise” “The Hermitage, Pontoise” 1876, private collection, Paris 1872, private collection, Japan Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #350 Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #529 41. La Moisson à Montfoucault La Meule, Pontoise “Harvest at Montfoucault” “The haystack, Pontoise” 1876, Musée d’Orsay (RF 3756) 1873, private collection, Paris Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #364 Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #223 42. Sous bois avec une femme assise Le Petit pont, Pontoise “In the woods” “Little bridge, Pontoise” 1876, private collection, Paris 1875, Kunsthalle, Mannheim Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #371 Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #300 43. Les Toits rouges, coin du village, effet d’hiver La Côte des boeufs à Pontoise “The red roofs” “La Côte des Boeufs, the Hermitage” 1877, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2735) 1877, National Gallery, London Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #384; Figure 2C Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #380; Figure 2D 44. Printemps à Pontoises, potager et Verger en fleurs, Louveciennes arbres en fleurs “Orchard in blossom, Louveciennes” “Orchard with flowering fruit trees, Pontoise” 1872, National Gallery, Washington 1877, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2733) Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #153 Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #387 45. Les Seigles, Pontoise, côte des Mathurins Gardeuse de vache sur la route du Chou, Pontoise or Les Orges “A cowherd at Pontoise” “Rye fields, Pontoise, seen from the Mathurins” 1874, Metropolitan Museum of Art, New York 1877, private collection, Japan Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #260 Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #406 46. Chemin sous-bois, en été Au repos sous-bois, Pontoise “Path through the woods” “Resting in the woods, Pontoise” 1877, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2731) 1878, Kunsthalle, Hamburg Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #416 Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #466 47. Lisière de bois or Clairière Le Châlet, la maison rose “Edge of the woods” “The chalet, the pink house” 1878, private collection, Paris 1870, Musée d’Orsay (RF 1937-58) Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #455 Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #82 48. Chemin montant à travers champs. Femme dans un clos Côte des Grouettes, Pontoise “Woman in an enclosure, spring “Path across the fields” sunshine in an Eragny field” 1879, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2736) 1887, Musée d’Orsay (RF 1937-47) Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #493 Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #709 49. La Brouette, verger Automne, l’étang de Monfoucault “The wheelbarrow” “Autumn, Monfoucault pond” ~1881, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2734) 1875, private collection, Paris Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #537 Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #329 Renoir 50. La Liseuse L’Amazone “Girl reading” “Madame Darras” 1874–1876, Musée d’Orsay (RF 3757) 1873, Musée d’Orsay (RF 1965-11) Daulte (1971) #106, Fezzi (1972) #202, Daulte (1971) #93, Fezzi (1972) #103 Wadley (1987) #36 51. La Place Saint-Georges La Mosquée, fête arabe “St. George Place” “The mosque” or “Arab festival in Algiers” ~1875, private collection 1881, Musée d’Orsay (RF 1957-8) Distel (1994) p.65, fig. 55 Fezzi (1972) #462, Hayward Gallery (1985) #56 342 CUTTING APPENDIX (Continued) 52. Étude, torse, effet de soleil Nue or Torse d’Anna “Torso of a woman in sunlight” “Female nude (Anna)” 1875–1876, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2740) 1979, Pushkin Art Museum, Moscow Daulte (1971) #201, Fezzi (1972) #204, Daulte (1971) #213, Fezzi (1972) #250 Hayward Gallery (1985) #36, Wadley (1987) #44 53. La Balançoire La Tonnelle du Moulin de la Galette “The swing” “The arbor” or “The bower” 1876, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2738) 1875–1876, Pushkin Art Museum, Moscow Daulte (1971) #202, Fezzi (1972) #242, Daulte (1971) #197, Fezzi (1972) # 240 Hayward Gallery (1985) #39 54. Bal du Moulin de la Galette, Montmartre‡ Le Déjeuner des canotiers “Ball at the Moulin de la Galette” “Luncheon of the boating party” 1876, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2740) 1881, Phillips Collection, Washington Daulte (1971) #209, Fezzi (1972) #249, Daulte (1971) #379, Fezzi (1972) #468, Hayward Gallery (1985) #40; Figure 2E Hayward Gallery (1985) #52; Figure 2F 55. Bords de Seine à Champrosay Paysage de neige “Banks of the Seine at Champrosay” “Snowy landscape” 1876, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2737) 1875, Musée de l’Orangerie, Paris Fezzi (1972) #256, Wadley (1987) #43 Hayward Gallery (1985) #33, Wadley (1987) #42 56. Le Pont du chemin de fer à Châtou Femme avec parasol dans le jardin “The railroad bridge at Châtou” “Woman with a parasol in the garden” 1881, Musée d’Orsay (RF 3758) 1873, Thyssen-Bornemisza Gallery, Madrid Fezzi (1972) #470 Fezzi (1972) #199, Wadley (1987) #22 57. Le Château des brouillards Chemin montant dans les hautes herbes or Soleil couchant à Montmartre “Path winding through tall grass” “The chateau of the mists” 1876–1877, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2581) (Renoir family home) Fezzi (1972) #141, Wadley (1987) #67 ~1890, private collection Distel (1994) p. 65, Figure 56 58. Jeunes filles au piano§ La Leçon de piano “Young girls at the piano” “Piano lesson” 1892, private collection, Paris ~1889, Joslyn Art Museum, Omaha Hayward Gallery (1985) #90 Daulte (1971) #561, Fezzi (1982) #661, Hayward Gallery (1985) #84 Sisley 59. Les Régates à Molesey Les régates à Hampton Court “Boat races at Molesey, near Hampton Court” “Boat races at Hampton Court” 1874, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2787) 1874, Sammlung E.G. Bührle, Zurich Daulte (1959) #126; Figure 2G Daulte (1959) #125; Figure 2H 60. Une Rue à Louveciennes Un Coin de bois aux Sablons “A street in Louveciennes” “A corner of the woods, the Sablons” ~1876, Musée de Beaux-Arts, Nice 1883, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2079) Daulte (1959) #221 (deaccessioned Daulte (1959) #502 from Musée d’Orsay, RF 2783) 61. La Seine à Suresnes Le Pont à Sèvres The Seine at Suresnes” or “The bridge at Sèvres” “The banks of the Seine” 1877, National Gallery, Prague 1877, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2786) Daulte (1959) #262 Daulte (1959) #267 62. Cour de ferme à Saint-Mammès Village de Voisins “Farmyard at Saint-Mammès” “Village of Voisins” 1884, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2700) 1874, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2019) Daulte (1959) #544 Daulte (1959) #142 63. Lisière de forêt au printemps À repos au bord du ruisseau “Edge of the forest near Fontainbleau” “Resting by a brook” 1885, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2784) 1872, Musée d’Orsay (RF 1693) Daulte (1959) #350 Daulte (1959) #42 MERE EXPOSURE HELPS MAINTAIN A CANON OF ART 343 APPENDIX (Continued) 64. Saint-Mammès Canal du Loing “Saint-Mammès” “Loing canal” 1885, Musée d’Orsay (RF 2785) 1884, Musée d’Orsay (RF 1972-33) Daulte (1959) #629 Daulte (1959) #522 65. Seine à Billancourt La Seine à Bougival “The Seine at Billancourt” “The Seine at Bougival” undated, private collection 1881, Musée d’Orsay (MNR 208) Distel (1994) p. 67, Figure 63 Daulte (1959) #90 66. Bords de la Seine, effet du soleil couchant Bateaux à l’écluse de Bougival “Banks of the Seine, sunset” “Boats at the Bougival lock” undated, private collection 1873, Musée d’Orsay (RF 1690) Distel (1994), p.67, Figure 59 Daulte (1959) #90 Caillebotte Images Not Found Monet Pissarro Une colline rose (Vues de Vétheuil) Coin de village – les choux “A pink hill in Vétheuil” “Village corner – cabbages” undated, location unknown 1875, location unknown not in Wildenstein (1974–1985) Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #312, image not presented Sisley La vallée en été, Pontoise “The valley in summer, Pontoise” Station de bateaux à Auteuil 1877, location unknown “Boat dock at Auteuil” Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #407, undated, location unknown image not presented not in Daulte (1959) Sous-bois en automne, Pontoise “In the autumn woods, Pontoise” 1879, location unknown Pissarro and Venturi (1939) #505, image not presented *Cézanne’s Image 7c is very similar to two others perhaps equally well known: One is in the New York Met- ropolitan Museum of Art (Venturi, 1936, #625), and the other is in the Art Institute of Chicago (Venturi, 1936, #626), both with the same name, “The gulf of Marseille, seen from Estaque.” †Manet’s Image 17c was a study for Courses à Longchamps, 1867, Art Institute of Chicago (Jamot & Wildenstein, 1932, #202). Image 17n is a similar study, but done several years later. ‡Renoir painted two versions of 54c. Caillebotte owned the original. For many years the smaller copy was on loan to the Metropolitan Museum of Art in New York (see Distel, 1990; White, 1984), but was sold at Christie’s to Ryoei Saito for $78 million in 1990. The week be- fore, Mr. Saito had purchased Van Gogh’s “Portrait of Dr. Gachet” for $82.5 million. Mr. Saito soon went bankrupt, and these two most expensive paintings ever sold (through mid 2003) are now probably in a Tokyo bank vault (Saltzman, 1998). §Under commission from the French state, Renoir painted at least five versions of 58c. That chosen by the government is now in the Musée d’Orsay (RF 755), another in the Musée de l’Orangerie, a third in the Metropolitan Museum, and at least a fourth in a private collection (Hayward Gallery, 1985, pp. 261–263; Wadley, 1987). (Manuscript received December 10, 2001; revision accepted for publication May 17, 2002.)